[Paleopsych] Economist: Pain perception: Sex and drugs
Premise Checker
checker at panix.com
Thu Aug 4 22:39:30 UTC 2005
Pain perception: Sex and drugs
http://www.economist.com/science/PrinterFriendly.cfm?Story_ID=4197761
Jul 21st 2005
Men and women seem to perceive pain in different ways. That may mean
they sometimes need different pain-relief drugs
MALES and females respond to pain differently, even as children. In
most places, boys are expected to show a stiff upper lip when they get
hurt, while in girls wailing is, well, girlie. In part, this
difference is learnt--or, at least, reinforced by learning. But
partly, it is innate. It is hard, for instance, to blame upbringing
for the finding that boy and girl babies show different responses to
pain six hours after birth, or that male rats are more long-suffering
than females. It is also life-long. Ed Keogh of the University of
Bath, in England, and his colleagues have found that women report
feeling pain in more bodily areas than men, and also feel it more
often over the course of their lives.
Many researchers are therefore concluding that genetics underpins at
least some of the difference, and that females really do feel pain
more than males. Indeed, some go further. They think that the way men
and women experience pain is not only quantitatively different, but
qualitatively different, too. In other words, men's and women's brains
process pain using different circuits. Some pain scientists therefore
think it is only a matter of time before painkillers are formulated
differently for men and women in order to account for this difference.
Jeffrey Mogil, director of the pain genetics laboratory at McGill
University in Montreal, is one of the leading advocates of such "pink
and blue" painkillers. Pick a disease at random, he says, and the
chances are that females and males will handle the pain associated
with it differently. That seems to be true in mice, at least. When new
mouse "models" of human disease are created by genetic engineers, Dr
Mogil and his colleagues are often asked to test the engineered mice
for their responses to pain. They consistently find differences in the
way the mutant, diseased mice and their non-mutation-carrying brethren
respond to painful stimuli. But, generally, those differences are seen
more strongly in one sex than the other.
A prescribing headache
The latest example of such a difference is in migraine, a condition
that is three times more common in women than in men. In 2004, a group
of researchers led by Michel Ferrari of Leiden University in the
Netherlands reported that they had created what they believed to be
the first mouse model of migraine. Since some researchers argue that
migraine is associated with heightened sensitivity to pain, they sent
their creation to Dr Mogil for testing. He stresses that his data are
preliminary. However, he does find a lowered pain threshold in the
mouse migraine model compared with healthy mice--but only in females.
Dr Mogil is now convinced that the pain response in men and women is
mediated by different brain circuits--and not only because of his own
observations. Obstetricians and gynaecologists have long known that
certain drugs are particularly effective in women. Mothers in
childbirth prefer nalbuphine to morphine, for instance. Men, however,
report the opposite preference when they are in pain.
Both nalbuphine and morphine work by stimulating the brain's
endogenous-opioid receptors (endogenous opioids are the molecules that
opium-derived drugs mimic). But opioid receptors come in several
varieties, two of the most important of which are known as mu and
kappa. Morphine binds to the mu receptors, while nalbuphine stimulates
the less well-studied kappa receptors. Kappa-receptor agonists, as
molecules such as nalbuphine are known, appear to have little or no
pain-relieving effect in men.
Two years ago, Dr Mogil identified the first gene known to be involved
in modulating pain thresholds in women. Variations in this gene have
no effect on men's responses to a kappa-receptor agonist called
pentazocine, but they do affect the response in women. The protein
produced by this gene, melanocortin-1 receptor, also affects hair and
skin colour. Working in collaboration with Roger Fillingim of the
University of Florida, Gainesville, Dr Mogil found that redheaded
women with fair skin--who have a particular version of the
receptor--have a heightened response to pentazocine.
Jon Levine and Robert Gear, of the National Institutes of Health Pain
Centre at the University of California, San Francisco, also think that
there are fundamental differences between the sexes when it comes to
pain. They have explored the effects of nalbuphine on post-operative
pain in men and women who have had their wisdom teeth removed. The
results suggest that kappa-opioid agonists not only fail to alleviate
pain in men, they can actually make it worse.
Dr Gear and Dr Levine believe that as well as an analgesia (ie,
pain-suppression) circuit, the brain contains what they call an
anti-analgesia circuit--one which, when activated, pumps pain up. They
have shown that which circuit is activated depends not only on the
type of receptor a drug acts on, but also the dose given. Among their
dental patients, low doses of nalbuphine had a short-lasting analgesic
effect in the women, but profoundly enhanced pain in the men. However,
when they added a low dose of naloxone--a drug that blocks all types
of opioid receptor--to the nalbuphine, the sex difference disappeared
and pain relief was significantly enhanced in everyone. After refining
the relative proportions of the two drugs in the mixture, they have
succeeded in finding (and patenting) a combination that is effective
in both sexes.
Nor is it only the mechanism of pain perception that differs between
the sexes. Dr Keogh and his colleagues argue that there are
significant differences in the ways men and women cope with pain, as
well.
This conclusion is based on studies involving hospital patients, as
well as others on volunteers who were exposed to a painful stimulus,
such as an ice-water arm-bath. Using this, the researchers were able
to measure the point at which people first notice pain, as well as
their tolerance--the point at which they can no longer stand it. Men
were able to minimise their experience of pain by concentrating on the
sensory aspects--their actual physical sensations. But this strategy
did not help women, who focused more on the emotional aspects. Since
the emotions associated with pain, such as fear and anxiety, tend to
be negative, the researchers suggest that the female approach may
actually exacerbate pain rather than alleviating it.
Dr Keogh, a psychologist, sees this difference as an effect of social
conditioning--and uses it to point up the dangers of under-estimating
social influences in favour of those of the genes. But it is not
obvious why such male and female "coping strategies" should not be
underpinned by genetics, in the same way that perceptions are.
The evolutionary reason why men resist pain better than women is,
however, a mystery. After all, pain is there to stop you doing bad
things to yourself. Perhaps it is because males and females are
exposed to different sorts of pain. Males, for instance, get into
fights much more often than females do, and thus get wounded more
often. On the other hand, they do not have to undergo the visceral
pain of childbirth. And perhaps a willingness to tolerate less pain
than men do helps to explain why women live longer than their menfolk.
More information about the paleopsych
mailing list