[Paleopsych] Denis Dutton on Madame Bovary's Ovaries
Premise Checker
checker at panix.com
Fri Aug 12 16:17:13 UTC 2005
Denis Dutton on Madame Bovary's Ovaries
http://denisdutton.com/barash_review.htm
Survival of the Fittest Characters
[1]Washington Post Book World, August 7, 2005
Denis Dutton
______________________________________________________________
[3]Madame Bovarys Ovaries: a Darwinian Look at Literature , by
David P. Barash and Nanelle R. Barash. New York: Delacourt Press,
2005, 272 pp. $24.00 paper, $32.00 cloth.
Human nature, evolved over millions of years and present in our
genes, expresses itself not only in bedrooms, boardrooms, and
battlefields but in creative human pursuits, including literature.
This, anyway, is the premise of an amusing, if over-ambitious, book
by psychologist/zoologist David P. Barash and his college-student
daughter, Nanelle.
The Barashes line up exemplary works of fiction from Homer to Saul
Bellow alongside the major claims of evolutionary psychology. The
prehistoric origins of human conduct and desires, so the idea goes,
should be able to tell us something about the conduct and values of
characters in fiction. The results are mixed: Some of the Barashes'
explanations are far-fetched, but others have the power to jolt us
into an altered view of familiar literary stories and characters.
Among the authors' best insights is their description of Jane
Austen's fiction in terms of sexual selection theory. Darwinian
evolution depends on natural selection: Unfit individuals die off
in a hostile environment, while the survivors pass their fitness on
to descendants. But for Darwin, there is also a second, parallel
and quite distinct process that drives evolution: sexual selection.
The heavy, cumbersome peacock's tail, far from helping the bird
survive, is a distinct hindrance, making peacocks more prone to
being eaten by predators. This remarkable tail is a product not of
natural, but of sexual selection: Peahens choose to mate with
peacocks sporting the most gorgeous feathers, which indicate both
healthy genes and the capacity to produce offspring with more
gorgeous feathers, increasing the likelihood that the mother's gene
line will survive into the future. By making discriminating mating
choices over thousands of generations, it is actually peahens, and
not their males, who by their choices have bred the peacock's tail.
Likewise, discriminating human females are central to the world of
Jane Austen, whom the Barashes call "the poet laureate of female
choice." Selecting a good mate is Austen's major theme. She is
particularly adept at bringing out, against the vast intricacies of
a social milieu, the basic values women seek in men, and men tend
to want in women (shortlist: good looks, health, money, status, IQ,
courage, dependability and a pleasant personality -- in many
different weightings and orderings). Not being a peacock, Mr. Darcy
does not have iridescent feathers, but for human females his
commanding personality, solid income, intelligence, generosity, and
the magnificent Pemberley estate do very nicely.
Cinderella is used to exemplify the well-known research of Martin
Daly and Margo Wilson showing that children are statistically at
much greater risk of murder or abuse by stepparents than by
biological parents. In this connection, the Barashes also discuss
Sarah Hrdy's study of the way dominant male langur monkeys kill the
infant offspring of rivals before mating with the infants' mothers.
In real life we may all know plenty of loving stepparents, but as
the Barashes explain, historical statistics are sadly on the side
of the European folk-tale tradition with its stereotype of the
wicked stepmother.
The battles of elephant seals are brought to bear on the rivalry
between Agamemnon and Achilles. The Barashes use evolutionary
principles to explain the tragic outrage of Othello in a world
whose double standard treats straying women much more severely than
philandering men. A discussion of John Steinbeck's portrayal of
male friendship in Of Mice and Men follows a clear and pertinent
analysis of reciprocity among animals. This includes a fascinating
account of the process by which a vampire bat unsuccessful in a
hunt can coax a well-fed fellow bat into vomiting up a meal of
blood. That too is friendship, maybe, though I learned from this
book more about vampire bats than about Steinbeck.
It is easy to make fun of animal analogies, but in fairness, the
Barashes are mostly modest and persuasive in drawing their
comparisons. Nevertheless, despite the authors' enthusiasm for
their subject, there is a curious flatness to Madame Bovary's
Ovaries.
First, the Barashes tend to pick and choose literary evidence as it
suits their case, a procedure generally verboten in research
psychology. They provide an adequate, if unsurprising, evolutionary
explanation of Emma Bovary's adultery (a female searching for
better genes). But what about another important event in the story,
Emma's suicide? Maybe there is an evolutionary explanation for
suicide as a solution for a person cornered in an intolerable
social situation, but it's not hinted at here.
At the same time, the authors also now and then claim for
evolutionary psychology more than the evidence warrants. Catcher in
the Rye is a tale of youthful alienation and rebellion. Parents,
we're told, push their children around, and "it makes perfect sense
that adolescents in particular are prone to fight back." Such
conflict is bound to occur between "every young individual and the
adult world that he or she must learn to negotiate." Fine, but
platitudes about Holden Caulfield's rebelliousness hardly need
validation by Darwin, and none is given here. The Barashes have
slipped into doing the most ordinary brand of criticism without
seeming to realize it.
In fact, Madame Bovary's Ovaries is less a Darwinian look at
literature than a discussion of evolutionary psychology that
happens to trawl through fiction for examples. If readers don't
know The Grapes of Wrath or the Iliad firsthand, they'll likely
have seen the movies or read the Cliffs Notes, which will be good
enough. The authors might as easily have clipped crime or human
interest stories from last month's newspapers, except that fiction
normally supplies interior monologues or narratives that reveal
motivations. This is a plus if you're trying to explain how evolved
psychology works.
But by reducing literature to a convenient collection of anecdotes
and case studies, the Barashes fail to engage broader features of
an expressive and communicative art. There is nothing here about
literary style, tone, and the crucial interaction between authors
and their audiences. From both a human and aesthetic perspective,
literature does not just report on what happened but shows us how
individuals make sense of what happened. It is about the beliefs,
attitudes, and modes of perception that distinguish us from each
other.
Literature also serves the human craving for novelty and surprise,
including twists and shocks that go against our normal, evolved
expectations and desires. The Barashes' approach can explain the
vicarious pleasure we might get in following the choices and
indecisions of a Jane Austen character as she settles on her man.
It can explain any story of a mother who fights to protect her
children from danger. But it has more trouble with the likes of a
Medea, who murders her children to satisfy her consuming hatred for
their father. The family story of Jason and Medea is one of the
most revoltingly entertaining soap operas in literature, exactly
because it perverts all expectations of a mother's normal conduct
toward her children.
David and Nanelle Barash wisely insist that they are not trying to
provide the decisive framework to explain literature. They give us
a few of the patterns of human behavior that contemporary science
can explain, showing that reproduction, survival and social
reciprocity are bread and butter topics of the fiction we love.
Yes, Sophocles, Shakespeare and Flaubert knew the human race at
least as well as any psychologist. The science in this book comes
out better than the literary criticism, but classic literature
remains, as ever, the ultimate winner.
David and Nanelle Barash have written an [4]entertaining piece for
the Chronicle of Higher Education summarizing their views. For
another treatment of the relation between Darwinism and literary
studies, take a look at the work of Joseph Carroll. I've reviewed
Carroll's latest book [5]here. If you have access to the Johns
Hopkins University Press journal [6]Philosophy and Literature
through your library, I'd also recommend an excellent article in
the latest issue. It's "[7]Literature and Evolution: a Bio-Cultural
Approach," by the Nabokov scholar and literary theorist, Brian
Boyd. -- D.D.
[8]Denis Dutton teaches philosophy of art at the University of
Canterbury in New Zealand.
References
1.
http://www.washingtonpost.com/wp-dyn/content/article/2005/08/04/AR2005080401595.html
2. http://www.denisdutton.com/
3.
http://www.amazon.com/exec/obidos/redirect?link_code=ur2&camp=1789&tag=denisduttonco-20&creative=9325&path=ASIN/0385338015/qid=1123382462/sr=2-1/ref=pd_bbs_b_ur_2_1
4. http://chronicle.com/temp/reprint.php?id=ma6s0ryboo4uyna4dh4g8219cnzrqk
5. http://denisdutton.com/carroll_review.htm
6. http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/philosophy_and_literature/toc/phl29.1.html
7. http://muse.jhu.edu/journals/philosophy_and_literature/v029/29.1boyd.html
8. mailto:constant.force at netaccess.co.nz
More information about the paleopsych
mailing list