[Paleopsych] CHE: Placebos Could Play a Role in Treating Some Conditions, Scientists Say

Premise Checker checker at panix.com
Fri Dec 2 02:55:19 UTC 2005


Placebos Could Play a Role in Treating Some Conditions, Scientists Say
News bulletin from the Chronicle of Higher Education, 5.11.21
http://chronicle.com/daily/2005/11/2005112103n.htm

    The placebo effect -- it's all in your head. When you swallow sugar
    pills instead of powerful medicine and your symptoms disappear, it's
    all thanks to the power of your mind.

    How does the brain perform that parlor trick? In the past, scientists
    suspected that any apparent health benefits from placebos had little
    more basis in biology than did sleight of hand. In studies of new
    drugs, patients might tell their doctors they feel better because they
    think that is what their doctors want to hear. Or perhaps they would
    have recovered without any treatment, real or sham.

    But researchers now know that the placebo effect is real and grounded
    in the physiology of the brain. Using techniques to peer inside the
    skull, they have begun to find regions of the brain that respond to
    placebos, and they have even watched a single nerve cell react to a
    sham medicine.

    Those studies show that placebos affect the brain in much the same way
    that actual treatments do, researchers reported here last week at the
    annual meeting of the Society for Neuroscience. In other words, the
    power to treat several troublesome disorders may be wrapped up in the
    three-pound spongy lump of tissue protected by the skull.

    The research points to the power of positive thinking -- even at the
    unconscious level. When the brain expects relief, it can manufacture
    some on its own. "The things you can change with a positive outlook
    are profound," said Tor D. Wager, an assistant professor of psychology
    at Columbia University. "They are deeper physiologically than we have
    previously appreciated."

    None of the researchers who study the mechanism of the placebo effect
    suggest that doctors should prescribe dummy pills instead of real
    medicine. But they say that the study of the placebo effect could
    change how scientists perform clinical trials of new treatments and
    could even alter how we understand and treat pain, Parkinson's
    disease, and depression.

    By studying placebos, said Christian S. Stohler, dean of the school of
    dentistry at the University of Maryland at Baltimore, "you crack into
    disease mechanisms that might be very important for improving the
    lives of many pain patients."

    Fooling the Patient

    Researchers gained their first glimpse at the causes of the placebo
    effect in the late 1970s, when scientists discovered that under
    certain conditions they could cancel the effect. In a study of pain
    relievers, a drug called naloxone prevented patients on placebo pills
    from experiencing the usual benefit. Since naloxone blocks the action
    of painkillers called opioids, researchers figured that placebos must
    stimulate the brain to produce its own opioids.

    In the 1990s, another set of experiments provided more evidence that
    the placebo effect was a real physiological phenomenon. Fabrizio
    Benedetti, a professor of neuroscience at the University of Turin, and
    others studied the effect without using a placebo.

    Dr. Benedetti judged that a placebo's effect comes from the patient's
    psychosocial context: talking to a doctor, observing the treatment,
    and expecting improved health. So he took away that context by giving
    study participants real drugs, but on the sly.

    Patients were told that they would receive an active drug, a placebo,
    or nothing through intravenous needles, and consented to get any of
    the different treatments without knowing when any treatment would be
    supplied. The scientists compared the results when a doctor overtly
    gave the patient the drug and when a computer supplied the drug
    without the patient's knowledge. Bedside manner, it turned out, made a
    difference: Patients required far more painkiller if they unknowingly
    received the medicine from a computer.

    When the doctor gives a drug in full view, Dr. Benedetti said at the
    neuroscience conference, "there is an additive effect of the drug and
    of the placebo, the psychosocial component."

    He suggested that his experimental setup could be extended to become
    part of the testing procedure for new drugs. Clinical trials could
    then compare covert and overt administration, rather than comparing
    the active drug to a placebo. That way, none of the volunteers would
    go through the trouble of participating without receiving the real
    experimental treatment, and researchers could still demonstrate that
    the drug was effective by showing that it reduced symptoms when given
    covertly.

    Peering at the Brain

    With the recent advent of modern brain-scanning techniques, scientists
    gained the ability to look directly at the regions of the brain
    involved in the placebo effect. In 2002 researchers in Finland and
    Sweden published in Science the first brain images of the effect,
    using a technique called positron emission tomography, better known as
    PET.

    The researchers pressed a hot surface onto the hands of nine male
    volunteers, and then a doctor gave them injections of either a
    painkiller or a placebo. When the researchers performed PET scans on
    the men, both the drug and the dummy induced high blood flow --
    indicating brain activity -- in an area of the brain called the
    rostral anterior cingulate cortex. That area plays a key role in the
    painkilling effects of opioid drugs.

    Then in 2004, also in Science, Mr. Wager reported using functional
    magnetic resonance imaging, or fMRI, to show that a placebo that
    relieved pain also decreased activity in the brain's pain-sensing
    areas.

    Different people felt varying amounts of pain relief from the placebo.
    The amount of pain reduction a volunteer experienced went hand in hand
    with the amount of change in activity in the brain.

    "Part of the effect of a drug," Mr. Wager said at the conference, "is
    it changes the way you think about drugs."

    Jon-Kar Zubieta, an associate professor of psychiatry and radiology at
    the University of Michigan at Ann Arbor, and several colleagues,
    including Dr. Stohler, of the University of Maryland, peered deeper
    into the brain's workings by finding out where the brain produces
    opioids in response to placebo treatment.

    They used PET scans along with a stain that marks opioid activity in
    the brain. When the researchers gave male volunteers a painful
    injection of saline solution into their jaw muscles, the scans showed
    an increase of opioids in the brain. Most of the regions where the
    brain produced painkillers coincided with the ones that Mr. Wager
    identified as important.

    "Expectation releases substances, molecules, in your brain that
    ultimately change your experience," said Dr. Stohler. "Our brain is on
    drugs. It's on our own drugs."

    Relief for Parkinson's

    The placebo effect helps not only people in pain but also patients
    with diseases. In fact, scientists got their most detailed look at the
    placebo effect by studying how single neurons responded to sham drugs
    given to Parkinson's patients.

    Parkinson's disease is a motor disorder caused by the loss of brain
    cells that produce dopamine. Some patients experience temporary relief
    of symptoms from a placebo, and a previous study showed that the
    relief occurred because the brain produced dopamine in response.

    Patients who have Parkinson's disease sometimes receive surgery to
    implant electrodes deep within the brain. The electrodes can stimulate
    a neuron or record its activity. Dr. Benedetti, of the University of
    Turin, and his colleagues enrolled 11 patients who underwent surgery
    for that type of treatment. They gave the patients a placebo
    injection, telling them it was a powerful drug that should improve
    their motor control. The researchers then compared the activity of a
    single neuron before and after injection of the placebo.

    In the six patients who responded to the placebo -- who demonstrated
    less arm rigidity and said they felt better -- the rate of firing of
    the neuron went down. (Nerve cells "fire," or generate electrical
    impulses, in order to send signals to neighboring neurons.) The
    neurons' firing rate did not change for people who experienced no
    placebo effect.

    Another disorder that shows clinical improvement with placebos is
    depression. Depressed patients' moods often lift when they take a
    placebo, although the effect does not last, and they normally need to
    seek real treatment, according to Helen S. Mayberg, a professor of
    neurology and of psychiatry and behavioral sciences at Emory
    University.

    Dr. Mayberg became immersed in placebo research a few years ago, when
    she did a PET study of the brain's response to an antidepressant and
    to a placebo.

    In her study of 15 depressed men, four who had taken Prozac and four
    who had received a placebo experienced a remission of their symptoms.
    At the end of six weeks, after allowing the drug sufficient time to
    take effect, Dr. Mayberg took PET scans. For patients whose symptoms
    improved, the regions where the brain activity increased after a
    patient took a placebo formed a subset of the regions that increased
    after a patient took the true drug.

    "Drug is placebo plus," she said at the conference.

    In patients whose symptoms did not improve, whether they were on
    Prozac or on the placebo, the brain activity did not increase in those
    regions.

    She published the results of that study in 2002, but at the conference
    she reported a new analysis of her data. In the study, she had also
    collected brain scans one week after patients had begun receiving
    their treatments, even though the drug had not yet taken its full
    effect.

    Still, people whose symptoms later improved, whether they took the
    placebo or Prozac, again had increased brain activity in similar
    areas. One week into treatment, she said, the men's state of mind
    could be interpreted as a "heightened state of expectation" since they
    were anticipating clinical improvements. Nonresponders did not show
    those patterns, so such expectation could be key to whether a
    depressed patient will recover.

    Raising Expectations

    Dr. Mayberg would like to find ways to help those who do not respond
    to antidepressant drugs, and she surmises that expectation could make
    the difference. Such patients, she said, perhaps should imagine
    themselves getting well. "What is expectation?" she asked. "How do you
    cultivate it?"

    Those are questions that all of the scientists involved in this
    research would like to answer. Patients with chronic pain, said Dr.
    Zubieta, of Michigan, perhaps have lost the ability to produce the
    brain's natural painkillers. "If you are able to recruit mechanisms
    that help you cope with stress or pain, that's a good thing," he said,
    "The question is, How do things like this, or meditation, or
    biofeedback, work? We don't know."

    Dr. Stohler, of Maryland, agrees. "Getting a person to boost their own
    machinery to improve health -- that's something that medicine needs to
    know," he said.

    It may be especially urgent for patients with dementia, according to
    Dr. Benedetti. At the conference, he reported preliminary results that
    patients with Alzheimer's disease may not experience placebo effects
    at all. He found that Alzheimer's patients felt no difference between
    overt and hidden administration of painkillers. To Dr. Benedetti, that
    suggests that the psychological components of treatments -- the
    expectation of health improvements, and the circuits that such
    expectations create in the brain -- are absent.

    Perhaps, he said at the conference, doctors need to take that loss
    into account when prescribing any drug for Alzheimer's patients. Those
    patients may need higher doses of many drugs, such as painkillers, if
    their brain has stopped aiding the drug's action.

    The mind, it seems, may play a critical role in treating diseases. And
    its services come free of charge, with no co-payments or deductibles.
      _________________________________________________________________

    Background articles from The Chronicle:
      * [68]Take 2 Herbal Remedies and Call Me in the Morning (11/18/2005)
      * [69]Pray and Bear It (2/11/2005)

    Magazine & Journal Reader:
      * [70]A Glance at 'Current Directions in Psychological Science': Why
        Placebos Work (10/19/2005)

   68. http://chronicle.com/weekly/v52/i13/13a01001.htm
   69. http://chronicle.com/weekly/v51/i23/23a00703.htm
   70. http://chronicle.com/daily/2005/10/2005101901j.htm

E-mail me if you have problems getting the referenced articles.



More information about the paleopsych mailing list