[Paleopsych] Geoffrey Miller: Cultural production and political ideology as courtship displays
Premise Checker
checker at panix.com
Tue Jan 4 00:29:58 UTC 2005
Cultural production and political ideology as courtship displays
http://www.unm.edu/~psych/faculty/political_peacocks.htm
[He's the author of The Mating Mind, a highly original book arguing how
sexual selection made men AND women human. This article came out before he
finished the book. This article should interest transhumanists, too, as
they contemplate post-humans. They should come in at least two sexes!]
Political peacocks
by Geoffrey F. Miller
published as:
Miller, G. F. (1996). Political peacocks. Demos Quarterly, 10
(Special issue on evolutionary psychology), pp. 9-11.
The puzzle
Suddenly, in the spring of 1986 in New York, hundreds of Columbia
University students took over the campus adminstration building and
demanded that the university sell off all of its stocks in companies
that do business in South Africa. As a psychology undergraduate at
Columbia, I was puzzled by the spontaneity, ardour, and near-unanimity
of the student demands for divestment. Why would mostly white, mostly
middle-class North Americans miss classes, risk jail, and occupy a
drab office building for two weeks, in support of political freedom
for poor blacks living in a country six thousand miles away? The
campus conservative newspaper ran a cartoon depicting the protest as
an annual springtime mating ritual, with Dionysian revels punctuated
by political sloganeering about this year's arbitrary cause. At the
time, I thought the cartoon tasteless and patronizing. Now, I wonder
if it contained a grain of truth. Although the protests achieved
their political aims only inefficiently and indirectly, they did
function very effectively to bring together young men and women who
claimed to share similar political ideologies. Everyone I knew was
dating someone they'd met at the sit-in. In many cases, the
ideological commitment was paper-thin, and the protest ended just in
time to study for semester exams. Yet the sexual relationships
facilitated by the protest sometimes lasted for years.
The hypothesis that loud public advertisements of one's political
ideology function as some sort of courtship display designed to
attract sexual mates, analogous to the peacock's tail or the
nightingale's song, seems dangerous. It risks trivializing all of
political discourse, just as the conservative cartoon lampooned the
Columbia anti-apartheid protests. The best way to avoid this pitfall
is not to ignore the sexual undertones to human political behavior,
but to analyze them seriously and respectfully using the strongest and
most relevant theory we have from evolutionary biology: Darwin's
theory of sexual selection through mate choice.
The history
Most people think of Darwinian evolution as a blind, haphazard,
unguided process in which physical environments impose capricious
selection pressures on species, which must adapt or die. True, for
natural selection itself. But Darwin himself seems to have become
rather bored with natural selection by the inanimate environment after
he published The Origin of Species in 1859. He turned to much more
interesting question of how animal and human minds can shape
evolution. In his 1862 book On the various contrivances by which
British and foreign orchids are fertilized by insects he outlined how
the perceptual and behavioral capacities of pollinators shape the
evolution of flower color and form. In his massive two-volume work of
1868, The variation of animals and plants under domestication, he
detailed how human needs and tastes have shaped the evolution of
useful and ornamental features in domesticates. Further works on
animal emotions in 1872 and the behavior of climbing plants in 1875
continued the trend towards an evolutionary psychology. Most
provocatively, Darwin combined the frisson of sex with the spookiness
of mind and the enigma of human evolution in his two-volume
masterpiece of 1871, The descent of man, and Selection in relation to
sex.
Darwin observed that many animals, especially females, are rather
picky about their sexual partners. But why would it ever pay to
reject a suitor? Being choosy requires time, energy, and intelligence
- costs that can impair survival. The basic rationale for mate choice
is that random mating is stupid mating. It pays to be choosy because
in a sexually reproducing species, the genetic quality of your mate
will determine half the genetic quality of your offspring. Ugly,
unhealthy mates usually lead to ugly, unhealthy offspring. By
forming a joint genetic venture with an attractive, high-quality mate,
one's genes are much more likely to be passed on. Mate choice is
simply the best eugenics and genetic screening that female animals are
capable of carrying out under field conditions, with no equipment
other than their senses and their brains.
Often, sexual selection through mate choice can lead to spectacular
results: the bowerbird's elaborate nest, the riflebird's riveting
dance, the nightingale's haunting song, and the peacock's iridescent
tail, for example. Such features are complex adaptations that evolved
through mate choice, to function both as advertisements of the male's
health and as aesthetic displays that excite female senses. One can
recognize these courtship displays by certain biological criteria:
they are expensive to produce and hard to maintain, they have survival
costs but reproductive benefits, they are loud, bright, rhythmic,
complex, and creative to stimulate the senses, they occur more often
after reproductive maturity, more often during the breeding season,
more often in males than in females, and more often when potential
mates are present than absent. Also, they tend to evolve according to
unpredictable fashion cycles that change the detailed structure and
content of the displays while maintaining their complexity, extremity,
and cost. By these criteria, most human behaviors that we call
cultural, ideological, and political would count as courtship
displays.
Victorian skeptics objected to Darwin's theory of sexual selection by
pointing out that in contemporary European society, women tended to
display more physical ornamentation than men, contrary to the
men-display-more hypothesis. This is true only if courtship display
is artificially restricted to physical artefacts worn on the body.
Whereas Victorian women ornamented themselves with mere jewelry and
clothing, men ornamented themselves with the books they wrote,
pictures they painted, symphonies they composed, country estates they
bought, honors they won, and vast political and economic empires they
built.
Although Darwin presented overwhelming evidence for his ingenious
sexual selection theory, it fell into disrepute for over a century.
Even Alfred Russell Wallace, the co-discoverer of natural selection,
preferred to view male ornaments as outlets for a surplus of male
energy, rather than as adaptations evolved through female choice. Even
now, we hear echoes of Wallace's fallacious surplus-of-energy argument
in most psychological and anthropological theories about the
"self-expressive" functions of human art, music, language, and
culture. The Modern Synthesis of Mendelian genetics and Darwinism in
the 1930s continued to reject female choice, assuming that sexual
ornaments simply intimidate other males or keep animals from mating
with the wrong species. Only in the 1980s, with a confluence of
support from mathematical models, computer simulations, and
experiments in animal and human mate choice, has Darwin's sexual
selection theory been re-established as a major part of evolutionary
biology. Unfortunately, almost everything written about the
evolutionary origins of the human mind, language, culture, ideology,
and politics, has ignored the power of sexual selection through mate
choice as a force that creates exactly these sorts of elaborate
display behaviors.
The hypothesis
Humans are ideological animals. We show strong motivations and
incredible capacities to learn, create, recombine, and disseminate
ideas. Despite the evidence that these idea-processing systems are
complex biological adaptations that must have evolved through
Darwinian selection, even the most ardent modern Darwinians such as
Stephen Jay Gould, Richards Dawkins, and Dan Dennett tend to treat
culture as an evolutionary arena separate from biology. One reason
for this failure of nerve is that it is so difficult to think of any
form of natural selection that would favor such extreme, costly, and
obsessive ideological behavior. Until the last 40,000 years of human
evolution, the pace of technological and social change was so slow
that it's hard to believe there was much of a survival payoff to
becoming such an ideological animal. My hypothesis, developed in a
long Ph.D. dissertation, several recent papers, and a forthcoming
book, is that the payoffs to ideological behavior were largely
reproductive. The heritable mental capacities that underpin human
language, culture, music, art, and myth-making evolved through sexual
selection operating on both men and women, through mutual mate
choice. Whatever technological benefits those capacities happen to
have produced in recent centuries are unanticipated side-effects of
adaptations originally designed for courtship.
Language, of course, is the key to ideological display. Whereas
songbirds can only toy with protean combinations of pitch, rhythm, and
timbre, language gives humans the closest thing to telepathy in
nature: the ability to transmit complex ideas from one head to
another, through the tricks of syntax and semantics. Language opens a
window into other minds, expanding the arena of courtship display from
the physical to the conceptual. This has enormous implications for
the way that sexual selection worked during the last few hundred
thousand years of human evolution. As human courtship relied more
heavily on language, mate choice focused more on the ideas that
language expresses. The selection pressures that shaped the evolution
of the human mind came increasingly not from the environment testing
whether one's hunting skills were sufficient for survival, but from
other minds testing whether one's ideas were interesting enough to
provoke some sexual attraction. Every ancestor of every human living
today was successful in attracting someone to mate with them.
Conversely, the millions of hominids and early humans who were too
dull and uninspiring to become our ancestors carried genes for brains
that were not as ideologically expressive as ours. A wonderful effect
of this runaway sexual selection was that brain size in our lineage
has tripled over the last two million years, giving us biologically
unprecedented capacities for creative thought, astonishing
expressiveness, and intricate culture. A more problematic effect is
that our ideological capacities were under selection to be novel,
interesting, and entertaining to other idea-infested minds, not to
accurately represent the external world or their own transient and
tangential place in it. This general argument applies to many domains
of human behaviour and culture, but for the remainder of the paper, I
will focus on political ideology.
The predictions and implications
The vast majority of people in modern societies have almost no
political power, yet have strong political convictions that they
broadcast insistently, frequently, and loudly when social conditions
are right. This behavior is puzzling to economists, who see clear
time and energy costs to ideological behavior, but little political
benefit to the individual. My point is that the individual benefits
of expressing political ideology are usually not political at all, but
social and sexual. As such, political ideology is under strong social
and sexual constraints that make little sense to political theorists
and policy experts. This simple idea may solve a number of old
puzzles in political psychology. Why do hundreds of questionnaires
show that men more conservative, more authoritarian, more
rights-oriented, and less empathy-oriented than women? Why do people
become more conservative as the move from young adulthood to middle
age? Why do more men than women run for political office? Why are most
ideological revolutions initiated by young single men?
None of these phenomena make sense if political ideology is a rational
reflection of political self-interest. In political, economic, and
psychological terms, everyone has equally strong self-interests, so
everyone should produce equal amounts of ideological behavior, if that
behavior functions to advance political self-interest. However, we
know from sexual selection theory that not everyone has equally strong
reproductive interests. Males have much more to gain from each act of
intercourse than females, because, by definition, they invest less in
each gamete. Young males should be especially risk-seeking in their
reproductive behavior, because they have the most to win and the least
to lose from risky courtship behavior (such as becoming a political
revolutionary). These predictions are obvious to any sexual selection
theorist. Less obvious are the ways in which political ideology is
used to advertise different aspects of one's personality across the
lifespan.
In unpublished studies I ran at Stanford University with Felicia
Pratto, we found that university students tend to treat each others'
political orientations as proxies for personality traits.
Conservatism is simply read off as indicating an ambitious,
self-interested personality who will excel at protecting and
provisioning his or her mate. Liberalism is read as indicating a
caring, empathetic personality who will excel at child care and
relationship-building. Given the well-documented, cross-culturally
universal sex difference in human mate choice criteria, with men
favoring younger, fertile women, and women favoring older,
higher-status, richer men, the expression of more liberal ideologies
by women and more conservative ideologies by men is not surprising.
Men use political conservatism to (unconsciously) advertise their
likely social and economic dominance; women use political liberalism
to advertise their nurturing abilities. The shift from liberal youth
to conservative middle age reflects a mating-relevant increase in
social dominance and earnings power, not just a rational shift in
one's self-interest.
More subtley, because mating is a social game in which the
attractiveness of a behavior depends on how many other people are
already producing that behavior, political ideology evolves under the
unstable dynamics of game theory, not as a process of simple
optimization given a set of self-interests. This explains why an
entire student body at an American university can suddenly act as if
they care deeply about the political fate of a country that they
virtually ignored the year before. The courtship arena simply
shifted, capriciously, from one political issue to another, but once a
sufficient number of students decided that attitudes towards apartheid
were the acid test for whether one's heart was in the right place, it
became impossible for anyone else to be apathetic about apartheid.
This is called frequency-dependent selection in biology, and it is a
hallmark of sexual selection processes.
What can policy analysts do, if most people treat political ideas as
courtship displays that reveal the proponent's personality traits,
rather than as rational suggestions for improving the world? The
pragmatic, not to say cynical, solution is to work with the evolved
grain of the human mind by recognizing that people respond to policy
ideas first as big-brained, idea-infested, hypersexual primates, and
only secondly as concerned citizens in a modern polity. This view
will not surprise political pollsters, spin doctors, and speech
writers, who make their daily living by exploiting our lust for
ideology, but it may surprise social scientists who take a more
rationalistic view of human nature. Fortunately, sexual selection
was not the only force to shape our minds. Other forms of social
selection such as kin selection, reciprocal altruism, and even group
selection seem to have favoured some instincts for political
rationality and consensual egalitarianism. Without the sexual
selection, we would never have become such colourful ideological
animals. But without the other forms of social selection, we would
have little hope of bringing our sexily protean ideologies into
congruence with reality.
Further Readings
Andersson, M. (1994). Sexual selection. Princeton U. Press.
Betzig, L. (1986). Despotism and differential reproduction: A
Darwinian view of history. Hawthorne, NY: Aldine.
Buss, D. M. (1994). The evolution of desire: Human mating
strategies. New York: Basic Books.
Cronin, H. (1991). The ant and the peacock: Altruism and sexual
selection from Darwin to today. Cambridge U. Press.
Darwin, C. (1871). The descent of man, and selection in relation
to sex (2 vols.). London: John Murray.
Fisher, H. (1992). Anatomy of love: The natural history of
monogamy, adultery, and divorce. New York: Simon & Schuster.
Miller, G. F. (1993). Evolution of the human brain through runaway
sexual selection: The mind as a protean courtship device. Ph.D.
thesis, Stanford University Psychology Department. (Available through
UMI Microfilms; Book in preparation for MIT Press/Bradford Books).
Miller, G. F. (in press). Sexual selection in human evolution: Review
and prospects. For C. Crawford & D. Krebs (Eds.), Evolution and Human
Behavior: Ideas, Issues, and Applications. Lawrence Erlbaum.
Miller, G. F., & Todd, P. M. (1995). The role of mate choice in
biocomputation: Sexual selection as a process of search,
optimization, and diversification. In W. Banzaf & F. Eeckman (Eds.),
Evolution and biocomputation: Computational models of evolution.
Lecture notes in computer science 899. (pp. 169-204).
Springer-Verlag.
Pomiankowski, A., & Moller, A. (1995). A resolution of the lek
paradox. Proc. R. Soc. London B, 260(1357), 21-29.
Ridley, M. (1993). The red queen: Sex and the evolution of human
nature. New York: Viking.
Wright, R. (1994). The moral animal: Evolutionary psychology and
everyday life. New York: Pantheon Books.
More information about the paleopsych
mailing list