[Paleopsych] NYTBR: Tool for Thought
Premise Checker
checker at panix.com
Sun Jan 30 18:42:01 UTC 2005
The New York Times > Books > Sunday Book Review > Essay: Tool for Thought
http://www.nytimes.com/2005/01/30/books/review/30JOHNSON.html
January 30, 2005
By STEVEN JOHNSON
One often hears from younger writers that they can't imagine how
anyone managed to compose an article, much less an entire book, with a
typewriter. Kerouac banging away at his Underwood portable? Hemingway
perched over his Remington? They might as well be monastic scribes or
cave painters.
But if the modern word processor has become a near-universal tool for
today's writers, its impact has been less revolutionary than you might
think. Word processors let us create sentences without the unwieldy
cross-outs and erasures of paper, and despite the occasional
catastrophic failure, our hard drives are better suited for storing
and retrieving documents than file cabinets. But writers don't
normally rely on the computer for the more subtle arts of inspiration
and association. We use the computer to process words, but the ideas
that animate those words originate somewhere else, away from the
screen. The word processor has changed the way we write, but it hasn't
yet changed the way we think.
Changing the way we think, of course, was the cardinal objective of
many early computer visionaries: Vannevar Bush's seminal 1945 essay
that envisioned the modern, hypertext-driven information machine was
called ''As We May Think''; Howard Rheingold's wonderful account of
computing's pioneers was called ''Tools for Thought.'' Most of these
gurus would be disappointed to find that, decades later, the most
sophisticated form of artificial intelligence in our writing tools
lies in our grammar checkers.
But 2005 may be the year when tools for thought become a reality for
people who manipulate words for a living, thanks to the release of
nearly a dozen new programs all aiming to do for your personal
information what Google has done for the Internet. These programs all
work in slightly different ways, but they share two remarkable
properties: the ability to interpret the meaning of text documents;
and the ability to filter through thousands of documents in the time
it takes to have a sip of coffee. Put those two elements together and
you have a tool that will have as significant an impact on the way
writers work as the original word processors did.
For the past three years, I've been using tools comparable to the new
ones hitting the market, so I have extensive firsthand experience with
the way the software changes the creative process. (I have used a
custom-designed application, created by the programmer Maciej
Ceglowski at the National Institute for Technology and Liberal
Education, and now use an off-the-shelf program called DEVONthink.)
The raw material the software relies on is an archive of my writings
and notes, plus a few thousand choice quotes from books I have read
over the past decade: an archive, in other words, of all my old ideas,
and the ideas that have influenced me.
Having all this information available at my fingerprints does more
than help me find my notes faster. Yes, when I'm trying to track down
an article I wrote many years ago, it's now much easier to retrieve.
But the qualitative change lies elsewhere: in finding documents I've
forgotten about altogether, documents that I didn't know I was looking
for.
What does this mean in practice? Consider how I used the tool in
writing my last book, which revolved around the latest developments in
brain science. I would write a paragraph that addressed the human
brain's remarkable facility for interpreting facial expressions. I'd
then plug that paragraph into the software, and ask it to find other,
similar passages in my archive. Instantly, a list of quotes would be
returned: some on the neural architecture that triggers facial
expressions, others on the evolutionary history of the smile, still
others that dealt with the expressiveness of our near relatives, the
chimpanzees. Invariably, one or two of these would trigger a new
association in my head -- I'd forgotten about the chimpanzee
connection -- and I'd select that quote, and ask the software to find
a new batch of documents similar to it. Before long a larger idea had
taken shape in my head, built out of the trail of associations the
machine had assembled for me.
Compare that to the traditional way of exploring your files, where the
computer is like a dutiful, but dumb, butler: ''Find me that document
about the chimpanzees!'' That's searching. The other feels different,
so different that we don't quite have a verb for it: it's riffing, or
brainstorming, or exploring. There are false starts and red herrings,
to be sure, but there are just as many happy accidents and unexpected
discoveries. Indeed, the fuzziness of the results is part of what
makes the software so powerful.
These tools are smart enough to get around the classic search engine
failing of excessive specificity: searching for ''dog'' and missing
all the articles that have only ''canine'' in them. Modern indexing
software learns associations between individual words, by tracking the
frequency with which words appear near each other. This can create
almost lyrical connections between ideas. I'm now working on a project
that involves the history of the London sewers. The other day I ran a
search that included the word ''sewage'' several times. Because the
software knows the word ''waste'' is often used alongside ''sewage''
it directed me to a quote that explained the way bones evolved in
vertebrate bodies: by repurposing the calcium waste products created
by the metabolism of cells.
That might seem like an errant result, but it sent me off on a long
and fruitful tangent into the way complex systems -- whether cities or
bodies -- find productive uses for the waste they create. It's still
early, but I may well get an entire chapter out of that little spark
of an idea.
Now, strictly speaking, who is responsible for that initial idea? Was
it me or the software? It sounds like a facetious question, but I mean
it seriously. Obviously, the computer wasn't conscious of the idea
taking shape, and I supplied the conceptual glue that linked the
London sewers to cell metabolism. But I'm not at all confident I would
have made the initial connection without the help of the software. The
idea was a true collaboration, two very different kinds of
intelligence playing off each other, one carbon-based, the other
silicon.
IF these tools do get adopted, will they affect the kinds of books and
essays people write? I suspect they might, because they are not as
helpful to narratives or linear arguments; they're associative tools
ultimately. They don't do cause-and-effect as well as they do ''x
reminds me of y.'' So they're ideally suited for books organized
around ideas rather than single narrative threads: more ''Lives of a
Cell'' and ''The Tipping Point'' than ''Seabiscuit.''
No doubt some will say that these tools remind them of the way they
use Google already, and the comparison is apt. (One of the new
applications that came out last year was Google Desktop -- using the
search engine's tools to filter through your personal files.) But
there's a fundamental difference between searching a universe of
documents created by strangers and searching your own personal
library. When you're freewheeling through ideas that you yourself have
collated -- particularly when you'd long ago forgotten about them --
there's something about the experience that seems uncannily like
freewheeling through the corridors of your own memory. It feels like
thinking.
Steven Johnson is the author, most recently, of ''Mind Wide Open.''
His new book, ''Everything Bad Is Good for You,'' will be published in
May.
More information about the paleopsych
mailing list