[Paleopsych] SW: On Synesthesia
Premise Checker
checker at panix.com
Tue Jul 26 19:45:10 UTC 2005
Neuroscience: On Synesthesia
http://scienceweek.com/2005/sw050722-2.htm
The following points are made by C. Mulvenna and V. Walsh (Current
Biology 2005 15:R399):
1) The term "synesthesia" refers to a phenomenon in which an
individual experiences a sense other than the one being stimulated.
This unusual pairing is automatic, present since childhood and
consistent across time. A specific experience will be activated by the
same stimulus with a seemingly arbitrary connection. For example, the
sight of the letter "q" may always activate the experience of a deep
red color; or a middle C played on a violin may always activate the
experience of the taste of tuna. The pairings can be more complex for
some synesthetes; for example, a sequence of pitches may activate the
sensation of gold, yellow and white moving rapidly upwards and at an
angle to the right, like a rippling stream . The condition is also
referred to as "sensory cross-activation".
2) Synesthesia does not apply to forced or acquired associations, such
as the word "Christmas" having connotations with the color red, the
smell of mince pies, or the general sound of Christmas carols. It also
does not include sensations triggering memories, such as a song
eliciting the memory of a person or place.
3) The first known reference to synesthesia in scientific writing is
John Locke's account of a blind man who described the color scarlet as
the sound of a trumpet in 1690. Similar isolated case-studies
continued for some time, and it was described in detail by Francis
Galton in 1883. Since then synesthesia has suffered repeated waves of
dismissal as a phantom condition, despite continual reports of its
existence. It is only relatively recently, with the application of
brain imaging techniques, that it has gained creditability in the
scientific world as a genuine neurological condition, and this
acceptance has led to the current surge in synesthesia research.
4) Sensory cross-activation in the brains of synesthetes has now been
observed by positron-emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic
resonance imaging (fMRI). Activation of brain regions associated with
visual perception was observed in blindfolded synesthetes listening to
words that evoked visual experiences. These activations were shown to
be clearly different from those evoked in either non-synesthetes or
the same synesthetes listening to tones that did not evoke visual
experiences. Activation of areas strongly associated with the
perception of color was observed in a group of word-color synesthetes.
This was not observed in non-synesthetes, even after they were trained
to associate pairings of words with colors. Current investigations are
examining if this neurological trend is observable across subtypes
involving other senses.
5) One theory suggests that, rather than synesthesia being caused by
extra connections "growing" between sensory areas, the apparent
cross-activation could be a result of reduced apoptosis which aids
differentiation of the sensory areas of the brain in the first months
after birth. Because of this increased sensory connectivity, some
experiences between certain senses in infancy may stay fixed in the
brain. If this is the case, we were all synesthetes at one stage, but
sensory modularity developed more explicitly in non-synesthetes.[1-5]
References (abridged):
1. Baron-Cohen, S. (1996). Is there a normal phase of synesthesia in
development?. Psyche. An Interdisciplinary Journal of Research on
Consciousness. Volume 2, number 27.
2. Baron-Cohen, S., Burt, L., Smith-Laittan, F., Harrison, J., and
Bolton, P. (1996). Synesthesia: Prevalence and similarity. Perception
25, 1073-1080
3. Baron-Cohen, S. and Harrison, J.E. (1997). In: Synesthesia: Classic
and contemporary readings.. (1997). Cambridge, Massachusetts:
Blackwell Publishers
4. Cohen-Kadosh, R., Sagiv, N., and Linden, D.E.J. (2005). When blue
is larger than red: colors influence numerical cognition in
synesthesia. J. Cogn. Neurosci., in press
5. Galton, F. (1883). Inquiries into human faculty and its
development. London Press
Current Biology http://www.current-biology.com
--------------------------------
Related Material:
THERMAL STIMULATION OF TASTE SENSATION
The term "chemoreceptors" refers to biological cells specialized to
respond to chemical stimuli, and the function of such a cell is to
signal to the nervous system a change in the chemical environment. In
humans, for example, major use of chemoreceptors occurs in those parts
of the body specialized for taste (gustatory sense) and smell
(olfaction). Taste receptors are found in the epithelium of the
tongue, and these receptors are responsible for sour, sweet, salty,
and bitter sensations from food applied to the tongue. Taste receptors
are also found in the pharynx and the upper part of the esophagus.
In contrast to olfactory receptors, taste receptors do not have their
own output extensions (axons) to send signals to the central nervous
system, but instead taste receptors stimulate the endings of nerve
fibers that send input to the central nervous system ("afferent
fibers"). Taste receptor cells are gathered into groups as "taste
buds", and the sensing of taste stimuli occurs in finger-like
projections (microvilli) at the surface of these taste buds, with
various chemical mechanisms proposed to account for transduction of
taste stimuli. In general, sourness depends primarily on the acidity
of a chemical stimulus, and salty sensations are evoked by solutions
with a high sodium concentration. Sweetness and bitterness, on the
other hand, are apparently transduced by specific *receptor cell
membrane receptors for sugars, amino acids, and other chemicals
Threshold concentrations for taste sensations produced by most
ingested substances are relatively high. For example, the threshold
concentration for sodium chloride is approximately 10 millimolar, for
sucrose, 20 millimolar, for citric acid 2 millimolar. The threshold is
much lower for certain bitter-tasting potentially dangerous plant
compounds: the threshold concentration for quinine is 0.008
millimolar, and for strychnine 0.0001 millimolar.
In humans, approximately 4000 taste buds are distributed throughout
the oral cavity and upper alimentary canal. Taste buds are
approximately 50 microns wide at their base and approximately 80
microns long, each bud containing 30 to 100 taste receptor cells.
Approximately 75 percent of all taste buds are found on the upper
(dorsal) surface of the tongue.
The following points are made by A. Cruz and B.G. Green (Nature 2000
403:889):
1) The authors point out that the first electrophysiological
recordings from animal and human taste nerves (in 1935 and 1985
respectively) provided clear evidence of thermal sensitivity, and
studies have indicated that as many as half the neurons in the
mammalian taste pathways respond to temperature. Since temperature has
never been shown to induce sensations of taste, it has been assumed
that thermal stimulation in the taste system is somehow nullified.
2) The authors report, however, that heating or cooling small areas of
the tongue can in fact cause sensations of taste: warming the front
(anterior) edge of the tongue (which is innervated by the chorda
tympani nerve) from an initially cold temperature can evoke sweetness,
whereas cooling can evoke sourness and/or saltiness. Thermal taste
also occurs on the rear of the tongue (which is innervated by the
glossopharyngeal nerve), but the relationship between temperature and
taste is different in that location from that found in the front of
the tongue.
3) The authors suggest these observations indicate the human taste
system contains several different types of thermally sensitive neurons
that normally contribute to the sensory code for taste, and that
although there is evidence for neurons whose chemosensitive mechanisms
are temperature sensitive, thermal sensitivity in some taste neurons
may arise from cellular processes unrelated to chemosensory
transduction.
Nature http://www.nature.com/nature
--------------------------------
Notes by ScienceWeek:
receptor cell membrane receptors: This phrase is a good illustration
of the two uses of the term "receptor" in cell biology. Biological
cells specialized to respond to specific physical or chemical stimuli
are called "receptors cells", or merely "receptors". However, specific
proteins or groups of proteins, embedded in the surface of a single
cell, and which respond to interactions with specific ions, chemical
groups, or molecules, and send molecular-level signals to the interior
of the cell, are also called "receptors".
--------------------------------
Related Material:
CROSS-MODALITY SENSATION
The following points are made by Alison Motluk (The Scientist 2001 11
Aug):
1) The classical and prevailing view of the brain holds that there are
5 separate senses feeding into 5 distinct brain regions genetically
wired to handle one and only one sense each. Sensory information is
thus parcelled up and analyzed in isolation. Recent research, however,
demonstrates that people who are born blind use the visual cortex when
they read Braille, and this has led to the idea that everyone has the
capacity to use non-classical regions for the analysis of sensory
information under certain circumstances, and that the brain is much
more versatile than many researchers have believed.
2) The brain is apparently able to quickly recruit new areas for
sensory analysis and also able to quickly reverse the recruitment,
with a time-scale apparently too short to involve new connections.
Tactile and auditory input into the visual cortex is apparently
present in all people and can be utilized for analysis if behaviorally
desirable.
3) Some researchers now believe that the brain is not organized into
specific sensory modalities, but instead it is split into units with
specific tasks or particular problems to solve, and these
task-oriented or problem-solving units simply use the most relevant
information available. The units may prefer certain senses at certain
times and under certain conditions, and prefer other senses at other
times. Vision, for example, may be the preferred way to judge
distances, but in the absence of vision, hearing or touch sensation
may be used to complete the same analysis.
The Scientist http://www.thescientist.com
More information about the paleopsych
mailing list