[Paleopsych] SW: On Social Selection for Eccentricity
Premise Checker
checker at panix.com
Sun May 22 22:09:46 UTC 2005
Evolution: On Social Selection for Eccentricity
http://scienceweek.com/2005/sa050211-1.htm
The following points are made by Michel Chapuisat (Current Biology
2004 14:1003):
1) In nature, colorful patterns usually constitute a signal; they may
deter competitors, frighten predators, or attract mates. The standard
view on animal signaling is that variation in ornamentation carries
information about the condition and quality of the signaler [1,2]. For
example, the black-and-yellow stripes of wasps are a signal of danger
to other species. But there is more to it than that. Recently,
Tibbetts [3] reported an experimental study showing that paper wasps
use intraspecific variation in facial and abdominal markings to
recognize individuals. A new comparative analysis by the same author
[4] has revealed that species with a flexible nest-founding strategy
have more variable markings than those with obligate single or
multiple foundresses. This new work suggests that complex social
interactions may select for individual distinctiveness and raises
interesting questions about the costs and benefits of revealing
individuality in social groups.
2) Polistes paper wasps form a widespread, species-rich group of
social insects [5]. They build small, open paper nests in protected
places. All paper wasps are eusocial: one or a few individuals
monopolize reproduction, while other individuals defend the colony,
forage, and care for the brood. After overwintering, mated females --
the queens -- found new nests. The species differ in their
nest-founding habits, following one of three possible strategies: they
may have an obligate single foundress, where only one queen starts a
nest; they may have obligate multiple foundresses, where two or more
queens start a nest together; or they may show flexible nest-founding,
where either a single queen or multiple queens start a nest.
3) Paper wasp colonies are well known for having a dominance hierarchy
[5]. In species with an obligate single foundress or obligate multiple
foundresses, dominant queens usually monopolize all reproduction, and
other females behave as workers. In species with a flexible
nest-founding strategy, the social interactions tend to be more
complex. There are even some theoretical and empirical indications
that queens engage in reproductive transactions whereby they yield
part of the reproductive potential to other females in order to make
them stay and cooperate peacefully. Complex alliances of this kind
require that wasps are able to accurately recognize individuals.
4) Polistes fuscatus individuals have highly variable markings on
their face and abdomen, such as the presence or absence of conspicuous
yellow eyebrows [3]. Together, these markings yield dozens of unique
patterns, suggesting they may serve for visual recognition of
individuals. Indeed, wasps that had experimentally altered markings
were found to receive more aggression than control wasps that had been
painted without altering their markings [3]. Importantly, the
aggression was transient and declined with time as wasps became
familiar with the new markings. This elegant study showed that wasps
use visual cues to distinguish individuals. Further, it suggested that
variable markings might undergo selection for improved individual
recognition in species with complex social interactions.
References (abridged):
1. Maynard Smith, J. and Harper, D. (2003). Animal signals. (Oxford:
Oxford University Press)
2. In Animal signals: signalling and signal design in animal
communication. (2000). Espmark, Y., Amundsen, T. and Rosenqvist, G.
eds. (Trondheim, Norway: Tapir, Academic Press)
3. Tibbetts, E.A. (2002). Visual signals of individual identity in the
wasp Polistes fuscatus. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 269, 1423-1428
4. Tibbetts, E.A. (2004). Complex social behavior can select for
variability in visual features: a case study in Polistes wasps. Proc.
R. Soc. Lond. B 271, 1955-1960
5. In Natural history and evolution of paper-wasps. (1996).
Turillazzi, S. and West-Eberhard, M.J. eds. (Oxford: Oxford University
Press)
Current Biology http://www.current-biology.com
--------------------------------
Related Material:
ON HONEYBEE SOCIAL BEHAVIOR, GENES, AND THE ENVIRONMENT
Notes by ScienceWeek:
The so-called social insects live in societies that rival human
societies in complexity and internal cohesion. Honey bees, for
example, apparently always follow 3 rules: a) they live in colonies
with overlapping generations; b) they care cooperatively for offspring
other than their own; and, c) they maintain a reproductive division of
labor.
The following points are made by Gene E. Robinson (American Scientist
1998 86:456):
1) Genes do not play an exclusive role in regulating behavior:
biologists have long realized that behavior is influenced by genes,
the environment, and interactions between the two.
2) Genes never act alone. They must operate in an environment where
they code for proteins that participate in many systems in an
organism, with these systems in turn influencing the expression of
genes. Consequently, biologists must take a broad approach in
assessing the impact of any gene.
3) The research group of the author uses the Western honey bee, Apis
mellifera. Honey bees pass through different life stages as they age,
and their behavioral responses to environmental and social stimuli
change in predictable ways. Although worker bees go through a
consistent path of behavioral development, this path is not rigidly
determined. Bees can accelerate, retard, or even reverse their
behavioral development in response to changing environmental and
colony conditions.
4) Experimental evidence indicates that juvenile hormone, one of the
most important hormones influencing insect development, helps time the
pace of behavioral maturation in honey bees. The rate of
endocrine-mediated behavioral development is influenced by inhibitory
social interactions. Older bees inhibit the behavioral development of
younger bees: the rate of behavioral development is negatively
correlated with the proportion of older bees in a colony. Inhibitory
social interactions that influence the rate of behavioral development
involve chemical communication between colony members.
5) Evidence from the laboratory of the author in 1993 indicated the
so-called mushroom bodies in the bee brain are involved in the
behavioral changes occurring during maturation, the volume of the
bodies increasing, and the volume increase associated with an increase
in synapses with neurons from brain regions devoted to sensory input.
The author suggests this was the first report of brain plasticity in
an invertebrate.
6) The author suggests that, in general, two-way interactions between
the nervous system and the genome contribute fundamentally to the
control of social behavior. Information about social conditions that
is acquired by the nervous system is likely to induce changes in
genomic function that in turn produce adaptive modifications of the
structure and function of the nervous system.
7) The author proposes a new research initiative called
"sociogenomics", defined as a "wide-ranging approach to identify genes
that influence social behavior, determining the influence of these
genes on underlying neural and endocrine mechanisms, and exploring the
effects of the environment -- particularly the social environment --
on gene action."
American Scientist http://www.americanscientist.org
--------------------------------
Related Material:
ON GENES AND COMPLEX SOCIAL BEHAVIOR
The following points are made by M.J. Krieger and K.G. Ross (Science
2002 295:328):
1) The evolution of complex social behavior is among the most
important events in the history of life. Interest in the genes
underlying the expression of key social traits is strong because
knowledge of the genetic architecture will lead to increasingly
realistic models of social evolution, while identification of the
products of major genes can elucidate the molecular bases of social
behavior. Few studies have succeeded in showing that complex social
behaviors have a heritable basis, and fewer still have suggested that
variation in these behaviors is attributable to the action of one or
few genes of major effect. No candidate genes with major effects on
key social polymorphisms have been identified previously.
2) The fire ant Solenopsis invicta displays a fundamental social
polymorphism that appears to be under simple genetic control. A basic
feature of fire ant colony social organization, the number of
egg-laying queens, is associated with variation at the gene Gp-9. In
the US, where this species has been introduced, colonies composed of
workers bearing only the (B) allele at Gp-9 invariably have a single
queen (monogyne social form), whereas colonies with workers bearing
the alternate (b) allele have multiple queens (polygyne social form).
The two social forms differ in many key reproductive and life history
characteristics.
3) The authors report they sequenced the gene Gp-9 and found that it
encodes a pheromone-binding protein, a crucial molecular component in
same-species (conspecific) chemical recognition. The authors suggest
this indicates that differences in worker Gp-9 genotypes between
social forms may cause differences in the abilities of workers to
recognize queens and regulate their numbers. The authors conclude:
"This study demonstrates that single genes of major effect can
underlie the expression of complex behaviors important in social
evolution."
More information about the paleopsych
mailing list