[Paleopsych] SW: On Generalized Anxiety Disorder
Premise Checker
checker at panix.com
Wed May 25 18:44:49 UTC 2005
Medical Biology: On Generalized Anxiety Disorder
http://scienceweek.com/2004/sa041022-3.htm
The following points are made by Gregory Fricchione (New Engl. J. Med.
2004 351:675):
1) Anxiety disorders are the most prevalent psychiatric conditions in
the US aside from disorders involving substance abuse.(1) Generalized
anxiety disorder has a lifetime prevalence of 5 percent. The onset is
usually before the age of 25 years, and the incidence in men is half
that in women. Untreated, the typical course is chronic, with a low
rate of remission and a moderate recurrence rate.(3)
2) Risk factors for generalized anxiety disorder include a family
history of the condition, an increase in stress, and a history of
physical or emotional trauma.(4,5) An association has also been
reported between smoking and anxiety, and the risk of generalized
anxiety disorder among adolescents who smoke heavily is five to six
times the risk among nonsmokers. Traits such as nervousness and social
discomfort may predispose people to both nicotine dependence and
anxiety. Medical illnesses are often associated with anxiety. For
example, generalized anxiety disorder occurs in 14 percent of patients
with diabetes.
3) Major depression is the most common coexisting psychiatric illness
in patients with generalized anxiety disorder, occurring in almost two
thirds of such patients. Panic disorder occurs in a quarter of
patients with generalized anxiety disorder, and alcohol abuse in more
than a third.(1) Studies of twins suggest a shared genetic propensity
to both generalized anxiety disorder and major depression, and a
recent report suggests that a genetic variant of the
serotonin-transporter gene may predispose people to both conditions.
In prospective studies, anxiety almost always appears to be the
primary disorder and to increase the risk of depression. Patients who
have generalized anxiety disorder along with coexisting psychiatric
illnesses have more impairment, seek more medical attention, and have
a poorer response to treatment than those without coexisting
illnesses.
4) Patients with generalized anxiety disorder often have physical
symptoms, and it may be difficult to distinguish the symptoms from
those of medical illnesses that are associated with anxiety. Factors
suggesting that anxiety is the symptom of a medical disorder include
an onset of the symptoms after the age of 35 years, no personal or
family history of anxiety, no increase in stress, little or no
avoidance of anxiety-provoking situations, and a poor response to
antianxiety medication. A physical cause should be suspected when
anxiety follows recent changes in medication or accompanies signs and
symptoms of a new disease.(2)
References (abridged):
1. Kessler RC, McGonagle KA, Zhao S, et al. Lifetime and 12-month
prevalence of DSM-III-R psychiatric disorders in the United States:
results from the National Comorbidity Survey. Arch Gen Psychiatry
1994;51:8-19
2. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th ed.:
DSM-IV. Washington, D.C.: American Psychiatric Association, 1994:435-6
3. Wittchen HU, Carter RM, Pfister H, Montgomery SA, Kessler RC.
Disabilities and quality of life in pure and comorbid generalized
anxiety disorder and major depression in a national survey. Int Clin
Psychopharmacol 2000;15:319-328
4. Brantley PJ, Mehan DJ Jr, Ames SC, Jones GN. Minor stressors and
generalized anxiety disorders among low-income patients attending
primary care clinics. J Nerv Ment Dis 1999;187:435-440
5. Brown ES, Fulton MK, Wilkeson A, Petty F. The psychiatric sequelae
of civilian trauma. Compr Psychiatry 2000;41:19-23
New Engl. J. Med. http://www.nejm.org
--------------------------------
Related Material:
ON SEROTONIN AND ANXIETY
The following points are made by Solomon H. Snyder (Nature 2002
416:377):
1) Perhaps one of the best known of neurotransmitters, serotonin has a
role in many different neurobiological processes. For example, it
helps to regulate our moods -- a fact that has been well established
since the 1950s, with the discovery that drugs that deplete serotonin
precipitate depression whereas increasing serotonin levels has
antidepressant effects. The idea that serotonin might also affect
anxiety was first suspected in the 1980s following the serendipitous
finding that buspirone, a drug developed to treat psychotic patients,
is also useful for treating anxiety disorders, and stimulates a type
of serotonin-detecting molecule in the body, the serotonin-1A
receptor. Later came the discovery that mice that have been
genetically engineered to lack this receptor, and so cannot respond
normally to serotonin, show increased "anxiety-like" behavior(2-4).
2) But the underlying mechanisms have been elusive. For instance, the
relevant brain regions have not been delineated. Moreover, the
findings in receptor-deficient mice appear to contradict observations
that compounds that block serotonin-1A receptors do not cause anxiety
in adult mice. Gross et al.(1) have substantially clarified these
issues: By using mice in which the serotonin-1A receptor can be
knocked out at will, they have shown that the absence of the receptor
in newborns does indeed lead to anxiety-like behavior, whereas its
knockout during adult life has no effect. Gross et al. also
discriminate between the role of the receptors in the hindbrain and in
forebrain structures such as the hippocampus and cerebral cortex.
3) Conventional gene-knockout techniques are powerful tools for
working out what a protein does. But they have major limitations
compared with using drugs (which might, for example, activate or
inhibit the protein of interest). Genes tend to be knocked out during
embryonic life, generally affecting the whole organism throughout its
lifetime. By contrast, a drug can be administered at any time and, in
the brain, can be injected into specific areas. The approach adopted
by Gross et al.1 is an ingenious way of addressing the shortcomings of
gene knockouts, providing time-and tissue-specific deletion and
restoration of serotonin-1A receptors. To achieve time-specific
knockouts, Gross et al. produced mice in which expression of the
serotonin-1A-receptor gene was under the control of the antibiotic
doxycycline. The gene could be switched off -- with a certain time lag
-- simply by feeding mice the antibiotic.
References (abridged):
1. Gross, C. et al. Nature 416, 396-400 (2002)
2. Parks, C. L., Robinson, P. S., Sibille, E., Shenk, T. & Toth, M.
Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 10734-10739 (1998)
3. Ramboz, S. et al. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 14476-14481 (1998)
4. Heisler, L. K. et al. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 95, 15049-15054
(1998)
5. D'Amato, R. J. et al. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 84, 4322-4326
(1987)
Nature http://www.nature.com/nature
--------------------------------
Related Material:
EMOTION, COGNITION, AND BEHAVIOR
The following points are made by R. J. Dolan (Science 2002 298:1191):
1) An ability to ascribe value to events in the world, a product of
evolutionary selective processes, is evident across phylogeny (1).
Value in this sense refers to an organism's facility to sense whether
events in its environment are more or less desirable. Within this
framework, emotions represent complex psychological and physiological
states that, to a greater or lesser degree, index occurrences of
value. It follows that the range of emotions to which an organism is
susceptible will, to a high degree, reflect on the complexity of its
adaptive niche. In higher order primates, in particular humans, this
involves adaptive demands of physical, socio-cultural, and
interpersonal contexts.
2) The importance of emotion to the variety of human experience is
evident in that what we notice and remember is not the mundane but
events that evoke feelings of joy, sorrow, pleasure, and pain. Emotion
provides the principal currency in human relationships as well as the
motivational force for what is best and worst in human behavior.
Emotion exerts a powerful influence on reason and, in ways neither
understood nor systematically researched, contributes to the fixation
of belief. A lack of emotional equilibrium underpins most human
unhappiness and is a common denominator across the entire range of
mental disorders from neuroses to psychoses, as seen, for example, in
obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) and schizophrenia. More than any
other species, we are beneficiaries and victims of a wealth of
emotional experience.
3) Progress in emotion research mirrors wider advances in cognitive
neurosciences where the idea of the brain as an information processing
system provides a highly influential metaphor. An observation by the
19th-century psychologist, William James (1842-1910), questions the
ultimate utility of a purely mind-based approach to human emotion.
James surmised that "if we fancy some strong emotion, and then try to
abstract from our consciousness of it all the feelings of its bodily
symptoms, we find we have nothing left behind, no mind-stuff out of
which the emotion can be constituted, and that a cold and neutral
state of intellectual perception is all that remains" (2). This
quotation highlights the fact that emotions as psychological
experiences have unique qualities, and it is worth considering what
these are. First, unlike most psychological states emotions are
embodied and manifest in uniquely recognizable, and stereotyped,
behavioral patterns of facial expression, comportment, and autonomic
arousal. Second, they are less susceptible to our intentions than
other psychological states insofar as they are often triggered, in the
words of James, "in advance of, and often in direct opposition of our
deliberate reason concerning them" (2). Finally, and most importantly,
emotions are less encapsulated than other psychological states as
evident in their global effects on virtually all aspects of cognition.
This is exemplified in the fact that when we are sad the world seems
less bright, we struggle to concentrate, and we become selective in
what we recall. These latter aspects of emotion and their influences
on other psychological functions are addressed here.
4) In summary: Emotion is central to the quality and range of everyday
human experience. The neurobiological substrates of human emotion are
now attracting increasing interest within the neurosciences, motivated
to a considerable extent by advances in functional neuroimaging
techniques. An emerging theme is the question of how emotion interacts
with and influences other domains of cognition, in particular
attention, memory, and reasoning. The psychological consequences and
mechanisms underlying the emotional modulation of cognition provide
the focus of much new research.(3-5)
References (abridged):
1. K. J. Friston, et al., Neuroscience 59, 229 (1994)
2. W. James, The Principles of Psychology (Holt, New York, 1890)
3. A. Ohman, et al., J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 130, 466 (2001)
4. J. L. Armony, et al., Neuropsychologia 40, 817 (2002)
5. K. Mogg, et al., Behav. Res. Ther. 35, 297 (1997)
Science http://www.sciencemag.org
More information about the paleopsych
mailing list