[Paleopsych] NYT: Expert Says He Discerns 'Hard-Wired' Grammar Rules
Premise Checker
checker at panix.com
Sun Oct 9 00:59:50 UTC 2005
Expert Says He Discerns 'Hard-Wired' Grammar Rules
New York Times, 2.1.15 (note date)
By BRENDA FOWLER
In 1981 the linguist Noam Chomsky, who had already proposed that language was
not learned but innate, made an even bolder claim.
The grammars of all languages, he said, can be described by a set of universal
rules or principles, and the differences among those grammars are due to a
finite set of options that are also innate.
If grammar were bread, then flour and liquid would be the universal rules; the
options -- parameters, Dr. Chomsky called them -- would be things like yeast,
eggs, sugar and jalapenos, any of which yield a substantially different product
when added to the universals. The theory would explain why grammars vary only
within a narrow range, despite the tremendous number and diversity of
languages.
While most linguists would now agree that language is innate, Dr. Chomsky's
ideas about principles and parameters have remained bitterly controversial.
Even his supporters could not claim to have tested his theory with the really
tough cases, the languages considered most different from those the linguists
typically know well.
But in a new book, Dr. Mark C. Baker, a linguist at Rutgers University whose
dissertation was supervised by Dr. Chomsky, says he has discerned the
parameters for a remarkably diverse set of languages, especially
American-Indian and African tongues.
In the book, "The Atoms of Language: The Mind's Hidden Rules of Grammar" (Basic
Books, 2001), Dr. Baker sets forth a hierarchy of parameters that sorts them
according to their power to affect and potentially nullify one another.
Just as the periodic table of elements illustrates the discrete units of the
physical world, Dr. Baker's hierarchy charts the finite set of discrete factors
that create differences in grammars.
That these parameters can be organized in a logical and systematic way, Dr.
Baker says, suggests that there may be some deeper theory underlying them, and
that the hierarchy may even guide language acquisition in children.
The hierarchy is not the same as a family tree, which illustrates the
historical relations among languages -- for example, Italian, French, Spanish
and their mother tongue, Latin. Nor does it have anything to do with the way
words vary from language to language. Instead, Dr. Baker analyzes grammar --
the set of principles that describe the order in which words and phrases are
strung together, tenses added and questions formed. Dr. Baker, like Dr.
Chomsky, believes these instructions are hard-wired into humans' brains.
His most spectacular discovery is that the grammars of English and Mohawk,
which appear radically different, are distinguished by just a single powerful
parameter whose position at the top of the hierarchy creates an enormous
effect.
Mohawk is a polysynthetic language: its verbs may be long and complicated, made
up of many different parts. It can express in one word what English must
express in many words. For example, "Washakotya'tawitsherahetkvhta'se' " means,
"He made the thing that one puts on one's body ugly for her" -- meaning, he
uglified her dress.
In that statement, "hetkv" is the root of the verb "to be ugly." Many of the
other bits are prefixes that specify the pronouns of the subject and object.
Every verb includes "each of the main participants in the event described by
the verb," Dr. Baker writes. In all, Mohawk has 58 prefixes, one for each
possible combination of subject, object and indirect object.
Dr. Baker says the polysynthesis parameter is the most fundamental difference
that languages can have, and it cleaves off Mohawk and a few other languages --
for example, Mayali, spoken in northern Australia -- from all others. That two
such far-flung languages operate in the same way is more evidence for the idea
that languages do not simply evolve in a gradual or unconstrained fashion, Dr.
Baker says.
At the next junction in the hierarchy, two parameters are at work: "optional
polysynthesis" (in which polysynthetic prefixes are possible, but not required)
and "head directionality," which dictates whether modifiers and other new words
are added before or after existing phrases. In English, new words are at the
front. For example, to make a prepositional phrase "with her sister," the
preposition goes before the noun. In Lakota, a Sioux language, the reverse is
true. The English sentence "I will put the book on the table" reads like this
in Lakota: "I table the on book the put will." Japanese, Turkish and
Greenlandic are other languages that opt for new words at the end of phrases,
while Khmer and Welsh have the same setting as English.
In all, Dr. Baker and others have identified about 14 parameters, and he
believes that there may be 16 more.
Dr. Baker's work is by no means universally accepted. Dr. Robert Van Valin, a
professor of linguistics at the State University of New York at Buffalo, says
the findings rest on a questionable assumption: that there is a universal
grammar.
"What they're doing in that whole program is taking English-like structures and
putting the words or parts of words of other languages in those structures and
then discovering that they're just like English," he said.
Dr. Karin E. Michelson, an associate professor of linguistics at SUNY Buffalo,
who also disagrees with the Chomskyan approach, said after reviewing Dr.
Baker's Mohawk work that some of the sentences he selected seemed artificial.
Dr. Baker acknowledged that some of the longer words in his study were
"carefully engineered," but he said the parameter still held up using more
common examples of Mohawk. He said using only examples from real discourse
restricted the kind of analysis that linguists could do.
"It would be like constraining a physicist to learn about gravity without ever
building a vacuum tube," Dr. Baker said.
Other linguists, however, say they are excited by Dr. Baker's work. "He's a
very influential linguist, and my guess is that this will provide insights and
will spawn research for the next few years," said Dr. Stephen Crain, a
professor of linguistics at the University of Maryland.
If Dr. Baker's theory is correct, a further question is how the parameters of
grammar are set as a child learns language. Does a child in an English-speaking
environment start at the top of the hierarchy, somehow grasp that polysynthesis
is not at work, and then move on to the next level in the hierarchy?
Dr. Baker also wonders why, if the brain is hard-wired for grammar, it leaves
the parameter settings unspecified. Why aren't they hard-wired, too?
Humans are assumed to have language in the first place because it allows them
to communicate useful information to others. But perhaps, Dr. Baker speculates,
language is also a tool of cryptography -- a way of concealing information from
competitors.
In that case, he went on, "the parameters would be the scrambling procedures."
More information about the paleopsych
mailing list