[Paleopsych] Prevention & Treatment: Listening to Prozac but Hearing Placebo

Premise Checker checker at panix.com
Sat Oct 22 02:08:43 UTC 2005


Listening to Prozac but Hearing Placebo: A Meta-Analysis of Antidepressant 
Medication
http://www.journals.apa.org/prevention/volume1/pre0010002a.html
Prevention & Treatment, Volume 1, Article 0002a, posted June 26, 1998

[I read something similar in Science, maybe twenty years ago, about the placebo 
effect being proportionate to the medical effect, and I think it deal with a 
much larger categories of illnesses. Does anyone know anything further about 
these anomalies?]

by Irving Kirsch, Ph.D., University of Connecticut, Storrs, CT
and Guy Sapirstein, Ph.D., Westwood Lodge Hospital, Needham, MA

      ABSTRACT

      Mean effect sizes for changes in depression were calculated for
      2,318 patients who had been randomly assigned to either
      antidepressant medication or placebo in 19 double-blind clinical
      trials. As a proportion of the drug response, the placebo response
      was constant across different types of medication (75%), and the
      correlation between placebo effect and drug effect was .90. These
      data indicate that virtually all of the variation in drug effect
      size was due to the placebo characteristics of the studies. The
      effect size for active medications that are not regarded to be
      antidepressants was as large as that for those classified as
      antidepressants, and in both cases, the inactive placebos produced
      improvement that was 75% of the effect of the active drug. These
      data raise the possibility that the apparent drug effect (25% of
      the drug response) is actually an active placebo effect.
      Examination of pre-post effect sizes among depressed individuals
      assigned to no-treatment or wait-list control groups suggest that
      approximately one quarter of the drug response is due to the
      administration of an active medication, one half is a placebo
      effect, and the remaining quarter is due to other nonspecific
      factors.
      _________________________________________________________________

      EDITORS' NOTE

      The article that follows is a controversial one. It reaches a
      controversial conclusion--that much of the therapeutic benefit of
      antidepressant medications actually derives from placebo
      responding. The article reaches this conclusion by utilizing a
      controversial statistical approach--meta-analysis. And it employs
      meta-analysis controversially--by meta-analyzing studies that are
      very heterogeneous in subject selection criteria, treatments
      employed, and statistical methods used. Nonetheless, we have chosen
      to publish the article. We have done so because a number of the
      colleagues who originally reviewed the manuscript believed it had
      considerable merit, even while they recognized the clearly
      contentious conclusions it reached and the clearly arguable
      statistical methods it employed.

      We are convinced that one of the principal aims of an electronic
      journal ought to be to bring our readers information on a variety
      of current topics in prevention and treatment, even though much of
      it will be subject to heated differences of opinion about worth and
      ultimate significance. This is to be expected, of course, when one
      is publishing material at the cutting-edge, in a cutting-edge
      medium.

      We also believe, however, that soliciting expert commentary to
      accompany particularly controversial articles facilitates the
      fullest possible airing of the issues most germane to appreciating
      both the strengths and the weaknesses of target articles. In the
      same vein, we welcome comments on the article from readers as well,
      though for obvious reasons, we cannot promise to publish all of
      them.

      Feel free to submit a comment by emailing admin at apa.org.

      Peter Nathan, Associate Editor (Treatment)
      Martin E. P. Seligman, Editor
      _________________________________________________________________

      We thank R. B. Lydiard and Smith-Kline Beecham Pharaceuticals for
      supplying additional data. We thank David Kenny for his assistance
      with the statistical analyses. We thank Roger P. Greenberg and
      Daniel E. Moerman for their helpful comments on earlier versions of
      this paper.

      Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to
      Irving Kirsch, Department of Psychology, U-20, University of
      Connecticut, 406 Babbidge Road Storrs, CT 06269-1020.
      E-mail: Irvingk at uconnvm.uconn.edu
      _________________________________________________________________

    More placebos have been administered to research participants than any
    single experimental drug. Thus, one would expect sufficient data to
    have accumulated for the acquisition of substantial knowledge of the
    parameters of placebo effects. However, although almost everyone
    controls for placebo effects, almost no one evaluates them. With this
    in mind, we set about the task of using meta-analytic procedures for
    evaluating the magnitude of the placebo response to antidepressant
    medication.

    Meta-analysis provides a means of mathematically combining results
    from different studies, even when these studies have used different
    measures to assess the dependent variable. Most often, this is done by
    using the statistic d, which is a standardized difference score. This
    effect size is generally calculated as the mean of the experimental
    group minus the mean of the control group, divided by the pooled
    standard deviation. Less frequently, the mean difference is divided by
    standard deviation of the control group (Smith, Glass, & Miller,
    1980).

    Ideally, to calculate the effect size of placebos, we would want to
    subtract the effects of a no-placebo control group. However, placebos
    are used as controls against which the effects of physical
    interventions can be gauged. It is rare for an experimental condition
    to be included against which the effects of the placebo can be
    evaluated. To circumvent this problem, we decided to calculate
    within-cell or pre-post effect sizes, which are the posttreatment mean
    depression score minus the pretreatment mean depression score, divided
    by the pooled standard deviation (cf. Smith et al., 1980). By doing
    this for both placebo groups and medication groups, we can estimate
    the proportion of the response to antidepressant medication that is
    duplicated by placebo administration, a response that would be due to
    such factors as expectancy for improvement and the natural course of
    the disorder (i.e., spontaneous remission). Later in this article, we
    also separate expectancy from natural history and provide estimates of
    each of these effects.

    Although our approach is unusual, in most cases it should provide
    results that are comparable to conventional methods. If there are no
    significant pretreatment differences between the treatment and control
    groups, then the subtraction of mean standardized pre-post difference
    scores should result in a mean effect size that is just about the same
    as that produced by subtracting mean standardized posttreatment
    scores. Suppose, for example, we have a study with the data displayed
    in Table 1. The conventionally calculated effect size would be would
    be 1.00. The pre-post effect sizes would be 3.00 for the treatment
    group and 2.00 for the control group. The difference between them is
    1.00, which is exactly the same effect calculated from posttreatment
    scores alone. However, calculating the effect size in this manner also
    provides us with the information that the effect of the control
    procedure was 2/3 that of the treatment procedure, information that we
    do not have when we only consider posttreatment scores. Of course, it
    is rare for two groups to have identical mean pretreatment scores, and
    to the extent that those scores are different, our two methods of
    calculation would provide different results. However, by controlling
    for baseline differences, our method should provide the more accurate
    estimate of differential outcome.

    CAPTION: Table 1
    Hypothetical Means and Standard Deviations for a Treatment Group and a
    Control Group

                       Treatment                  Control
              Pretreatment Posttreatment Pretreatment Posttreatment
           M         25.00         10.00        25.00         15.00
           SD         5.50          4.50         4.50          5.50

                    The Effects of Medication and Placebo

Study Characteristics

    Studies assessing the efficacy of antidepressant medication were
    obtained through previous reviews (Davis, Janicak, & Bruninga, 1987;
    Free & Oei, 1989; Greenberg & Fisher, 1989; Greenberg, Bornstein,
    Greenberg, & Fisher, 1992; Workman & Short, 1993), supplemented by a
    computer search of PsycLit and MEDLINE databases from 1974 to 1995
    using the search terms drug-therapy or pharmacotherapy or
    psychotherapy or placebo and depression or affective disorders.
    Psychotherapy was included as a search term for the purpose of
    obtaining articles that would allow estimation of changes occurring in
    no-treatment and wait-list control groups, a topic to which we return
    later in this article. Approximately 1,500 publications were produced
    by this literature search. These were examined by the second author,
    and those meeting the following criteria were included in the
    meta-analysis:

     1. The sample was restricted to patients with a primary diagnosis of
        depression. Studies were excluded if participants were selected
        because of other criteria (eating disorders, substance abuse,
        physical disabilities or chronic medical conditions), as were
        studies in which the description of the patient population was
        vague (e.g., "neurotic").
     2. Sufficient data were reported or obtainable to calculate
        within-condition effect sizes. This resulted in the exclusion of
        studies for which neither pre-post statistical tests nor
        pretreatment means were available.
     3. Data were reported for a placebo control group.
     4. Participants were assigned to experimental conditions randomly.
     5. Participants were between the ages of 18 and 75.

    Of the approximately 1,500 studies examined, 20 met the inclusion
    criteria. Of these, all but one were studies of the acute phase of
    therapy, with treatment durations ranging from 1 to 20 weeks (M =
    4.82). The one exception (Doogan & Caillard, 1992) was a maintenance
    study, with a duration of treatment of 44 weeks. Because of this
    difference, Doogan and Caillard's study was excluded from the
    meta-analysis. Thus, the analysis was conducted on 19 studies
    containing 2,318 participants, of whom 1,460 received medication and
    858 received placebo. Medications studied were amitriptyline,
    amylobarbitone, fluoxetine, imipramine, paroxetine, isocarboxazid,
    trazodone, lithium, liothyronine, adinazolam, amoxapine, phenelzine,
    venlafaxine, maprotiline, tranylcypromine, and bupropion.

   The Calculation of Effect Sizes

    In most cases, effect sizes (d) were calculated for measures of
    depression as the mean posttreatment score minus the mean pretreatment
    score, divided by the pooled standard deviation (SD). Pretreatment SDs
    were used in place of pooled SDs in calculating effect sizes for four
    studies in which posttreatment SDs were not reported (Ravaris, Nies,
    Robinson, et al., 1976; Rickels & Case, 1982; Rickels, Case,
    Weberlowsky, et al., 1981; Robinson, Nies, & Ravaris, 1973). The
    methods described by Smith et al. (1980) were used to estimate effect
    sizes for two studies in which means and SDs were not reported. One of
    these studies (Goldberg, Rickels, & Finnerty, 1981) reported the t
    value for the pre-post comparisons. The effect size for this study was
    estimated using the formula:

      d= t (2/n)^1/2

    where t is the reported t value for the pre-post comparison, and n is
    the number of subjects in the condition. The other study (Kiev &
    Okerson, 1979) reported only that there was a significant difference
    between pre- and posttreatment scores. As suggested by Smith et al.
    (1980), the following formula for estimating the effect size was used:

      d= 1.96 (2/n)^ 1/2 ,

    where 1.96 is used as the most conservative estimation of the t value
    at the .05 significance level used by Kiev and Okerson. These two two
    effect sizes were also corrected for pre-post correlation by
    multiplying the estimated effect size by (1 - r)^ 1/2 , r being the
    estimate of the test-retest correlation (Hunter & Schmidt, 1990).
    Bailey and Coppen (1976) reported test-retest correlations of .65 for
    the Beck Depression Inventory (BDI; Beck, Ward, Mendelson, Mock, &
    Erbaugh, 1961) and .50 for the Hamilton Rating Scale for Depression
    (HRS-D; Hamilton, 1960) . Therefore, in order to arrive at an
    estimated effect size, corrected for the pre-post correlation, the
    estimated effect sizes of the HRS-D were multiplied by 0.707 and the
    effect sizes of the BDI were multiplied by 0.59.

    In studies reporting multiple measures of depression, an effect size
    was calculated for each measure and these were then averaged. In
    studies reporting the effects of two drugs, a single mean effect size
    for both was calculated for the primary analysis. In a subsequent
    analysis, the effect for each drug was examined separately. In both
    analyses, we calculated mean effect sizes weighted for sample size (D;
    Hunter & Schmidt, 1990).

   Effect Sizes

    Sample sizes and effect sizes for patients receiving medication or
    placebo are presented in Table 2. Mean effect sizes, weighted for
    sample size, were 1.55 SDs for the medication response and 1.16 for
    the placebo response. Because effect sizes are obtained by dividing
    both treatment means by a constant (i.e., the pooled SD), they can be
    treated mathematically like the scores from which they are derived. ^1
    In particular, we have shown that, barring pretreatment between-group
    differences, subtracting the mean pre-post effect size of the control
    groups from the mean pre-post effect size of the experimental groups
    is equivalent to calculating an effect size by conventional means.
    Subtracting mean placebo response rates from mean drug response rates
    reveals a mean medication effect of 0.39 SDs. This indicates that 75%
    of the response to the medications examined in these studies was a
    placebo response, and at most, 25% might be a true drug effect. This
    does not mean that only 25% of patients are likely to respond to the
    pharmacological properties of the drug. Rather, it means that for a
    typical patient, 75% of the benefit obtained from the active drug
    would also have obtained from an inactive placebo.

    CAPTION: Table 2
    Studies Including Placebo Control Groups

                                             Drug    Placebo
                          Study             n   d    n    d
                Blashki et al. (1971)       43 1.75  18  1.02
                Byerly et al. (1988)        44 2.30  16  1.37
                Claghorn et al. (1992)     113 1.91  95  1.49
                Davidson & Turnbull (1983)  11 4.77   8  2.28
                Elkin et al. (1989)         36 2.35  34  2.01
                Goldberg et al. (1981)     179 0.44  93  0.44
                Joffe et al. (1993)         34 1.43  16  0.61
                Kahn et al. (1991)          66 2.25  80  1.48
                Kiev & Okerson (1979)       39 0.44  22  0.42
                Lydiard (1989)              30 2.59  15  1.93
                Ravaris et al. (1976)       14 1.42  19  0.91
                Rickels et al. (1981)       75 1.86  23  1.45
                Rickels & Case (1982)      100 1.71  54  1.17
                Robinson et al. (1973)      33 1.13  27  0.76
                Schweizer et al. (1994)     87 3.13  57  2.13
                Stark & Hardison (1985)    370 1.40 169  1.03
                van der Velde (1981)        52 0.66  27  0.10
                White et al. (1984)         77 1.50  45  1.14
                Zung (1983)                 57  .88  40  0.95

    Inspection of Table 2 reveals considerable variability in drug and
    placebo response effect sizes. As a first step toward clarifying the
    reason for this variability, we calculated the correlation between
    drug response and placebo response, which was found to be
    exceptionally high, r = .90, p < .001 (see Figure 1). This indicates
    that the placebo response was proportionate to the drug response, with
    remaining variability most likely due to measurement error.

      [pre0010002afig1a.gif]

      Figure 1. The placebo response as a predictor of the drug response.

    Our next question was the source of the common variability. One
    possibility is that the correlation between placebo and drug response
    rates are due to between-study differences in sample characteristics
    (e.g., inpatients vs. outpatients, volunteers vs. referrals, etc.).
    Our analysis of psychotherapy studies later in this article provides a
    test of this hypothesis. If the correlation is due to between-study
    differences in sample characteristics, a similar correlation should be
    found between the psychotherapy and no-treatment response rates. In
    fact, the correlation between the psychotherapy response and the
    no-treatment response was nonsignificant and in the opposite
    direction. This indicates that common sample characteristics account
    for little if any of the relation between treatment and control group
    response rates.

    Another possibility is that the close correspondence between placebo
    and drug response is due to differences in so-called nonspecific
    variables (e.g., provision of a supportive relationship, color of the
    medication, patients' expectations for change, biases in clinician's
    ratings, etc.), which might vary from study to study, but which would
    be common to recipients of both treatments in a given study.
    Alternately, the correlation might be associated with differences in
    the effectiveness of the various medications included in the
    meta-analysis. This could happen if more effective medications
    inspired greater expectations of improvement among patients or
    prescribing physicians (Frank, 1973; Kirsch, 1990). Evans (1974), for
    example, reported that placebo morphine was substantially more
    effective than placebo aspirin. Finally, both factors might be
    operative.

    We further investigated this issue by examining the magnitude of drug
    and placebo responses as a function of type of medication. We
    subdivided medication into four types: (a) tricyclics and
    tetracyclics, (b) selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRI), (c)
    other antidepressants, and (d) other medications. This last category
    consisted of four medications (amylobarbitone, lithium, liothyronine,
    and adinazolam) that are not considered antidepressants.

    Weighted (for sample size) mean effect sizes of the drug response as a
    function of type of medication are shown in Table 3, along with
    corresponding effect sizes of the placebo response and the mean effect
    sizes of placebo responses as a proportion of drug responses. These
    data reveal relatively little variability in drug response and even
    less variability in the ratio of placebo response to drug response, as
    a function of drug type. For each type of medication, the effect size
    for the active drug response was between 1.43 and 1.69, and the
    inactive placebo response was between 74% and 76% of the active drug
    response. These data suggest that the between-drug variability in drug
    and placebo response was due entirely to differences in the placebo
    component of the studies.

    CAPTION: Table 3
    Effect Sizes as a Function of Drug Type

    Statistic Type of drug
    Antidepressant Other
    drugs
    Tri- and
    tetracyclic SSRI Other
    N 1,353 626 683 203
    K 13 4 8 3
    D--Drug 1.52 1.68 1.43 1.69
    D--Placebo 1.15 1.24 1.08 1.29
    Placebo/drug .76 .74 .76 .76
    N = number of subjects; K = number of studies; D = mean weighted
    effect size; placebo/drug = placebo response as a proportion of active
    drug response.

    Differences between active drug responses and inactive placebo
    responses are typically interpreted as indications of specific
    pharmacologic effects for the condition being treated. However, this
    conclusion is thrown into question by the data derived from active
    medications that are not considered effective for depression. It is
    possible that these drugs affect depression indirectly, perhaps by
    improving sleep or lowering anxiety. But if this were the case and if
    antidepressants have a specific effect on depression, then the effect
    of these other medications ought to have been less than the effect of
    antidepressants, whereas our data indicate that the response to these
    nonantidepressant drugs is at least as great as that to conventional
    antidepressants.

    A second possibility is that amylobarbitone, lithium, liothyronine,
    and adinazolam are in fact antidepressants. This conclusion is
    rendered plausible by the lack of understanding of the mechanism of
    clinical action of common antidepressants (e.g., tricyclics). If the
    classification of a drug as an antidepressant is established by its
    efficacy, rather than by knowledge of the mechanism underlying its
    effects, then amylobarbitone, lithium, liothyronine, and adinazolam
    might be considered specifics for depression.

    A third possibility is that these medications function as active
    placebos (i.e., active medications without specific activity for the
    condition being treated). Greenberg and Fisher (1989) summarized data
    indicating that the effect of antidepressant medication is smaller
    when it is compared to an active placebo than when it is compared to
    an inert placebo (also see Greenberg & Fisher, 1997). By definition,
    the only difference between active and inactive placebos is the
    presence of pharmacologically induced side effects. Therefore,
    differences in responses to active and inert placebos could be due to
    the presence of those side effects. Data from other studies indicate
    that most participants in studies of antidepressant medication are
    able to deduce whether they have been assigned to the drug condition
    or the placebo condition (Blashki, Mowbray, & Davies, 1971; Margraf,
    Ehlers, Roth, Clark, Sheikh, Agras, & Taylor, 1991; Ney, Collins, &
    Spensor, 1986).^ This is likely to be associated with their previous
    experience with antidepressant medication and with differences between
    drug and placebo in the magnitude of side effects. Experiencing more
    side effects, patients in active drug conditions conclude that they
    are in the drug group; experiencing fewer side effects, patients in
    placebo groups conclude that they are in the placebo condition. This
    can be expected to produce an enhanced placebo effect in drug
    conditions and a diminished placebo effect in placebo groups. Thus,
    the apparent drug effect of antidepressants may in fact be a placebo
    effect, magnified by differences in experienced side effects and the
    patient's subsequent recognition of the condition to which he or she
    has been assigned. Support for this interpretation of data is provided
    by a meta-analysis of fluoxetine (Prozac), in which a correlation of
    .85 was reported between the therapeutic effect of the drug and the
    percentage of patients reporting side effects (Greenberg, Bornstein,
    Zborowski, Fisher, & Greenberg, 1994).

                           Natural History Effects

    Just as it is important to distinguish between a drug response and a
    drug effect, so too is it worthwhile to distinguish between a placebo
    response and a placebo effect (Fisher, Lipman, Uhlenhuth, Rickels, &
    Park, 1965). A drug response is the change that occurs after
    administration of the drug. The effect of the drug is that portion of
    the response that is due to the drug's chemical composition; it is the
    difference between the drug response and the response to placebo
    administration. A similar distinction can be made between placebo
    responses and placebo effects. The placebo response is the change that
    occurs following administration of a placebo. However, change might
    also occur without administration of a placebo. It may be due to
    spontaneous remission, regression toward the mean, life changes, the
    passage of time, or other factors. The placebo effect is the
    difference between the placebo response and changes that occur without
    the administration of a placebo (Kirsch, 1985, 1997).

    In the preceding section, we evaluated the placebo response as a
    proportion of the response to antidepressant medication. The data
    suggest that at least 75% of the drug response is a placebo response,
    but it does not tell us the magnitude of the placebo effect. What
    proportion of the placebo response is due to expectancies generated by
    placebo administration, and what proportion would have occurred even
    without placebo administration? That is a much more difficult question
    to answer. We have not been able to locate any studies in which pre-
    and posttreatment assessments of depression were reported for both a
    placebo group and a no-treatment or wait-list control group. For that
    reason, we turned to psychotherapy outcome studies, in which the
    inclusion of untreated control groups is much more common.

    We acknowledge that the use of data from psychotherapy studies as a
    comparison with those from drug studies is far less than ideal.
    Participants in psychotherapy studies are likely to differ from those
    in drug studies on any number of variables. Furthermore, the
    assignment of participants to a no-treatment or wait-list control
    group might also effect the course of their disorder. For example,
    Frank (1973) has argued that the promise of future treatment is
    sufficient to trigger a placebo response, and a wait-list control
    group has been conceputalized as a placebo control group in at least
    one well-known outcome study (Sloane, Staples, Cristol, Yorkston, &
    Whipple, 1975). Conversely, one could argue that being assigned to a
    no-treatment control group might strengthen feelings of hopelessness
    and thereby increase depression. Despite these problems, the
    no-treatment and wait-list control data from psychotherapy outcome
    studies may be the best data currently available for estimating the
    natural course of untreated depression. Furthermore, the presence of
    both types of untreated control groups permits evaluation of Frank's
    (1973) hypothesis about the curative effects of the promise of
    treatment.

   Study Characteristics

    Studies assessing changes in depression among participants assigned to
    wait-list or no-treatment control groups were obtained from the
    computer search described earlier, supplemented by an examination of
    previous reviews (Dobson, 1989; Free, & Oei, 1989; Robinson, Berman, &
    Neimeyer, 1990). The publications that were produced by this
    literature search were examined by the second author, and those
    meeting the following criteria were included in the meta-analysis:

     1. The sample was restricted to patients with a primary diagnosis of
        depression. Studies were excluded if participants were selected
        because of other criteria (eating disorders, substance abuse,
        physical disabilities or chronic medical conditions), as were
        studies in which the description of the patient population was
        vague (e.g., "neurotic").
     2. Sufficient data were reported or obtainable to calculate
        within-condition effect sizes.
     3. Data were reported for a wait-list or no-treatment control group.
     4. Participants were assigned to experimental conditions randomly.
     5. Participants were between the ages of 18 and 75.

    Nineteen studies were found to meet these inclusion criteria, and in
    all cases, sufficient data had been reported to allow direct
    calculation of effect sizes as the mean posttreatment score minus the
    mean pretreatment score, divided by the pooled SD. Although they are
    incidental to the main purposes of this review, we examined effect
    sizes for psychotherapy as well as those for no-treatment and
    wait-list control groups.

   Effect Sizes

    Sample sizes and effect sizes for patients assigned to psychotherapy,
    wait-list, and no-treatment are presented in Table 4. Mean pre-post
    effect sizes, weighted for sample size, were 1.60 for the
    psychotherapy response and 0.37 for wait-list and no-treatment control
    groups. Participants given the promise of subsequent treatment (i.e.,
    those in wait-list groups) did not improve more than those not
    promised treatment. Mean effect sizes for these two conditions were
    0.36 and 0.39, respectively. The correlation between effect sizes (r =
    -.29) was not significant.

    CAPTION: Table 4
    Studies Including Wait-List or No-Treatment
    Control Groups

                       Study               Psychotherapy  Control
                                         n        d       n    d
            Beach & O'Leary (1992)       15          2.37 15  0.97
            Beck & Strong (1982)         20          2.87 10 -0.28
            Catanese et al. (1979)       99          1.39 21  0.16
            Comas-Diaz (1981)            16          1.87 10 -0.12
            Conoley & Garber (1985)      38          1.10 19  0.21
            Feldman et al. (1982)        38          2.00 10  0.42
            Graff et al. (1986)          24          2.03 11 -0.03
            Jarvinen & Gold (1981)       46          0.76 18  0.34
            Maynard (1993)               16          1.06 14  0.36
            Nezu (1986)                  23          2.39  9  0.16
            Rehm et al. (1981)           42          1.23 15  0.48
            Rude (1986)                   8          1.75 16  0.74
            Schmidt & Miller (1983)      34          1.25 10  0.11
            Shaw (1977)                  16          2.17  8  0.41
            Shipley & Fazio (1973)       11          2.12 11  1.00
            Taylor & Marshall (1977)     21          1.94  7  0.27
            Tyson & Range (1981)         22          0.67 11  1.45
            Wierzbicki & Bartlett (1987) 18          1.17 20  0.21
            Wilson et al. (1983)         16          2.17  9 -0.02

   Comparison of Participants in the Two Groups of Studies

    Comparisons of effect sizes from different sets of studies is common
    in meta-analysis. Nevertheless, we examined the characteristics of the
    samples in the two types of studies to assess their comparability.
    Eighty-six percent of the participants in the psychotherapy studies
    were women, as were 65% of participants in the drug studies. The age
    range of participants was 18 to 75 years (M = 30.1) in the
    psychotherapy studies and 18 to 70 years (M = 40.6) in the drug
    studies. Duration of treatment ranged from 1 to 20 weeks (M = 4.82) in
    psychotherapy studies and from 2 to 15 weeks (M = 5.95) in
    pharmacotherapy studies. The HRS-D was used in 15 drug studies
    involving 2,016 patients and 5 psychotherapy studies with 191
    participants. Analysis of variance weighted by sample size did not
    reveal any significant differences in pretreatment HRS-D scores
    between patients in the drug studies (M = 23.93, SD = 5.20) and
    participants in the psychotherapy studies (M = 21.34, SD = 5.03). The
    Beck Depression Inventory (BDI) was used in 4 drug studies involving
    261 patients and in 17 psychotherapy studies with 677 participants.
    Analysis of variance weighted by sample size did not reveal any
    significant differences in pretreatment BDI scores between
    participants in drug studies (M = 21.58, SD = 8.23) and those in
    psychotherapy studies (M = 21.63, SD = 6.97). Thus, participants in
    the two types of studies were comparable in initial levels of
    depression. These analyses also failed to reveal any pretreatment
    differences as a function of group assignment (treatment or control)
    or the interaction between type of study and group assignment.

   Estimating the Placebo Effect

    Just as drug effects can be estimated as the drug response minus the
    placebo response, placebo effects can be estimated as the placebo
    response minus the no-treatment response. Using the effect sizes
    obtained from the two meta-analyses reported above, this would be 0.79
    (1.16 - 0.37). Figure 2 displays the estimated drug, placebo, and
    no-treatment effect sizes as proportions of the drug response (i.e.,
    1.55 SDs). These data indicate that approximately one quarter of the
    drug response is due to the administration of an active medication,
    one half is a placebo effect, and the remaining quarter is due to
    other nonspecific factors.

      [pre0010002afig2a.gif]

      Figure 2. Drug effect, placebo effect, and natural history effect
      as proportions of the response to antidepressant medication.

                                  Discussion

    No-treatment effect sizes and effect sizes for the placebo response
    were calculated from different sets of studies. Comparison across
    different samples is common in meta-analyses. For example, effect
    sizes derived from studies of psychodynamic therapy are often compared
    to those derived from studies of behavior therapy (e.g., Andrews &
    Harvey, 1981; Smith et al., 1980). Nevertheless, comparisons of this
    sort should be interpreted cautiously. Participants volunteering for
    different treatments might come from a different populations, and when
    data for different conditions are drawn from different sets of
    studies, participants have not been assigned randomly to these
    conditions. Also, assignment to a no-treatment or wait-list control
    group is not the same as no intervention at all. Therefore, our
    estimates of the placebo effect and natural history component of the
    response to antidepressant medication should be considered tentative.
    Nevertheless, when direct comparisons are not available, these
    comparisons provide the best available estimates of comparative
    effectiveness. Furthermore, in at least some cases, these estimates
    have been found to yield results that are comparable to those derived
    from direct comparisons of groups that have been randomly assigned to
    condition (Kirsch, 1990; Shapiro & Shapiro, 1982).

    Unlike our estimate of the effect of natural history as a component of
    the drug response, our estimate of the placebo response as a
    proportion of the drug response was derived from studies in which
    participants from the same population were assigned randomly to drug
    and placebo conditions. Therefore, the estimate that only 25% of the
    drug response is due to the administration of an active medication can
    be considered reliable. Confidence in the reliability of this estimate
    is enhanced by the exceptionally high correlation between the drug
    response and the placebo response. This association is high enough to
    suggest that any remaining variance in drug response is error variance
    associated with imperfect reliability of measurement. Examining
    estimates of active drug and inactive placebo responses as a function
    of drug type further enhances confidence in the reliability of these
    estimates. Regardless of drug type, the inactive placebo response was
    approximately 75% of the active drug response.

    We used very stringent criteria in selecting studies for inclusion in
    this meta-analysis, and it is possible that data from a broader range
    of studies would have produced a different outcome. However, the
    effect size we have calculated for the medication effect (D = .39) is
    comparable to those reported in other meta-analyses of antidepressant
    medication (e.g., Greenberg et al., 1992, 1994; Joffe, Sokolov, &
    Streiner, 1996; Quality Assurance Project, 1983; Smith et al., 1980;
    Steinbrueck, Maxwell, & Howard, 1983). Comparison with the Joffe et
    al. (1996) meta-analysis is particularly instructive, because that
    study, like ours, included estimates of pre-post effect sizes for both
    drug and placebo. Although only two studies were included in both of
    these meta-analyses and somewhat different calculation methods were
    used, ^2 their results were remarkably similar to ours. They reported
    mean pre-post effect sizes of 1.57 for medication and 1.02 for placebo
    and a medication versus placebo effect size of .50.

    Our results are in agreement with those of other meta-analyses in
    revealing a substantial placebo effect in antidepressant medication
    and also a considerable benefit of medication over placebo. They also
    indicate that the placebo component of the response to medication is
    considerably greater than the pharmacological effect. However, there
    are two aspects of the data that have not been examined in other
    meta-analyses of antidepressant medication. These are (a) the
    exceptionally high correlation between the placebo response and the
    drug response and (b) the effect on depression of active drugs that
    are not antidepressants. Taken together, these two findings suggest
    the possibility that antidepressants might function as active
    placebos, in which the side-effects amplify the placebo effect by
    convincing patients of that they are receiving a potent drug.

    In summary, the data reviewed in this meta-analysis lead to a
    confident estimate that the response to inert placebos is
    approximately 75% of the response to active antidepressant medication.
    Whether the remaining 25% of the drug response is a true pharmacologic
    effect or an enhanced placebo effect cannot yet be determined, because
    of the relatively small number of studies in which active and inactive
    placebos have been compared (Fisher & Greenberg, 1993). Definitive
    estimates of placebo component of antidepressant medication will
    require four arm studies, in which the effects of active placebos,
    inactive placebos, active medication, and natural history (e.g.,
    wait-list controls) are examined. In addition, studies using the
    balanced placebo design would be of help, as these have been shown to
    diminish the ability of subjects to discover the condition to which
    they have been assigned (Kirsch & Rosadino, 1993).

                                  References

    Andrews, G., & Harvey, R. (1981). Does psychotherapy benefit neurotic
    patients? A reanalysis of the Smith, Glass, and Miller data. Archives
    of General Psychiatry, 36, 1203-1208.
    Bailey, J., & Coppen, A. (1976). A comparison between the Hamilton
    Rating Scale and the Beck Depression Inventory in the measurement of
    depression . British Journal of Psychiatry, 128, 486-489.
    Beach, S. R. H., & O'Leary, K. D. (1992). Treating depression in the
    context of marital discord: Outcome and predictors of response of
    marital therapy versus cognitive therapy. Behavior Therapy, 23,
    507-528.
    Beck, J. T., & Strong, S. R. (1982). Stimulating therapeutic change
    with interpretations: A comparison of positive and negative
    connotation. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 29(6), 551-559.
    Beck, A.T., Ward, C.H., Mendelson, M., Mock, J., & Erbaugh, J. (1961).
    An inventory for measuring depression. Archives of General Psychiatry,
    4, 561-571.
    Blashki, T. G., Mowbray, R., & Davies, B. (1971). Controlled trial of
    amytriptyline in general practice. British Medical Journal, 1,
    133-138.
    Byerley, W. F., Reimherr, F. W., Wood, D. R., & Grosser, B. I. (1988).
    Fluoxetine, a selective serotonine uptake inhibitor for the treatment
    of outpatients with major depression. Journal of Clinical
    Psychopharmacology, 8, 112-115.
    Catanese, R. A., Rosenthal, T. L., & Kelley, J. E. (1979). Strange
    bedfellows: Reward, punishment, and impersonal distraction strategies
    in treating dysphoria. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 3(3), 299-305.
    Claghorn, J. L., Kiev, A., Rickels, K., Smith, W. T., & Dunbar, G. C.
    (1992). Paroxetine versus placebo: A double-blind comparison in
    depressed patients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 53(12), 434-438.
    Comas-Diaz, L. (1981). Effects of cognitive and behavioral group
    treatment on the depressive symptomatology of Puerto Rican women.
    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 49(5), 627-632.
    Conoley, C. W., & Garber, R. A. (1985). Effects of reframing and
    self-control directives on loneliness, depression, and
    controllability. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 32(1), 139-142.
    Davidson, J., & Turnbull, C. (1983). Isocarboxazid: Efficacy and
    tolerance. Journal of Affective Disorders, 5, 183-189.
    Davis, J. M., Janicak, P. G., & Bruninga, K. (1987). The efficacy of
    MAO inhibitors in depression: A meta-analysis. Psychiatric Annals,
    17(12), 825-831.
    Dobson, K. S. (1989). A meta-analysis of the efficacy of cognitive
    therapy for depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology,
    57(3), 414-419.
    Doogan, D. P., & Caillard, V. (1992). Sertaline in the prevention of
    depression. British Journal of Psychiatry, 160, 217-222.
    Elkin, I., Shea, M. T., Watkins, J. T., Imber, S. D., Sotsky, S. M.,
    Collins, J. F., Glass, D. R., Pilkonis, P. A., Leber, W. R., Docherty,
    J. P., et al. (1989). National Institute of Mental Health, Treatment
    of Depression Collaborative Research Program: General effectiveness of
    treatments. Archives of General Psychiatry, 46(11), 971-982.
    Evans, F. J. (1974). The placebo response in pain reduction. In J. J.
    Bonica (Ed.), Advances in neurology: Vol. 4. Pain (pp. 289-296). New
    York: Raven.
    Feldman, D. A., Strong, S. R., & Danser, D. B. (1982). A comparison of
    paradoxical and nonparadoxical interpretations and directives. Journal
    of Counseling Psychology, 29, 572-579.
    Fisher, S., Lipman, R.S., Uhlenhuth, E.H., Rickels, K., and Park, L.C.
    (1965). Drug effects and initial severity of symptomatology.
    Psychopharmacologia, 7, 57-60.
    Fisher, S., & Greenberg, R. P. (1993). How sound is the double-blind
    design for evaluating psychiatric drugs? Journal of Nervous and Mental
    Disease, 181, 345-350.
    Frank, J. D. (1973). Persuasion and healing (rev. ed.). Baltimore:
    Johns Hopkins.
    Free, M. L., & Oei, T. P. S. (1989). Biological and psychological
    processes in the treatment and maintenance of depression. Clinical
    Psychology Review, 9, 653-688.
    Goldberg, H. L., Rickels, K., & Finnerty, R. (1981). Treatment of
    neurotic depression with a new antidepressant. Journal of Clinical
    Psychopharmacology, 1(6), 35S-38S (Supplement).
    Graff, R. W., Whitehead, G. I., & LeCompte, M. (1986). Group treatment
    with divorced women using cognitive-behavioral and supportive-insight
    methods. Journal of Counseling Psychology, 33, 276-281.
    Greenberg, R. P., Bornstein, R. F., Greenberg, M. D., & Fisher, S.
    (1992). A meta-analysis of antidepressant outcome under "blinder"
    conditions. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 60,
    664-669.
    Greenberg, R.P., Bornstein, R.F., Zborowski, M.J., Fisher, S., &
    Greenberg, M.D. (1994). A meta-analysis of fluoxetine outcome in the
    treatment of depression. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 182,
    547-551.
    Greenberg, R. P., & Fisher, S. (1989). Examining antidepressant
    effectiveness: Findings, ambiguities, and some vexing puzzles. In S.
    Fisher & R. P. Greenberg (Eds.) The limits of biological treatments
    for psychological distress. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
    Greenberg, R. P., & Fisher, S. (1997). Mood-mending medicines: Probing
    drug, psychotherapy, and placebo solutions. In S. Fisher & R. P.
    Greenberg (Eds.), From placebo to panacea: Putting psychiatric drugs
    to the test (pp. 115-172). New York: Wiley.
    Hamilton, M. A. (1960). A rating scale for depression. Journal of
    Neurology, Neurosurgery, and Psychiatry, 23, 56-61.
    Hedges, L. V., & Olkin, I. (1995). Statistical methods for
    meta-analysis. Orlando, FL: Academic Press.
    Hunter, J. E., & Schmidt, F. L. (1990). Methods of meta-analysis:
    Correcting error and bias in research findings. Newbury Park, CA:
    Sage.
    Jarvinen, P. J., & Gold, S. R. (1981). Imagery as an aid in reducing
    depression. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 37(3), 523-529.
    Joffe, R. T., Singer, W., Levitt, A. J., & MacDonald, C. (1993). A
    placebo controlled comparison of lithium and triiodothyronine
    augmentation of tricyclic antidepressants in unipolar refractory
    depression. Archives of General Psychiatry, 50, 387-393.
    Joffe, R., Sokolov, S., & Streiner, D. (1996). Antidepressant
    treatment of depression: A metaanalysis. Canadian Journal of
    Psychiatry, 41, 613-616.
    Khan, A., Dager, S. R., Cohen, S., et al. (1991). Chronicity of
    depressive episode in relation to antidepressant-placebo response.
    Neuropsychopharmacology, 4, 125-130.
    Kiev, A., & Okerson, L. (1979). Comparison of the therapeutic efficacy
    of amoxapine with that of imipramine: A controlled clinical study in
    patients with depressive illness. Clinical Trials Journal, 16(3),
    68-72.
    Kirsch, I. (1985). Response expectancy as a determinant of experience
    and behavior. American Psychologist, 40, 1189-1202.
    Kirsch, I. (1990). Changing expectations: A key to effective
    psychotherapy. Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/Cole.
    Kirsch, I. (1997). Specifying nonspecifics: Psychological mechanisms
    of placebo effects. In A. Harrington (Ed.), The placebo effect: An
    interdisciplinary exploration (pp. 166-186). Cambridge, MA: Harvard
    University Press.
    Kirsch, I., & Rosadino, M. J. (1993). Do double-blind studies with
    informed consent yield externally valid results? An empirical test.
    Psychopharmacology, 110, 437-442.
    Lydiard, R. B. et al. (1989). Fluvoxamine, imipramine and placebo in
    the treatment of depressed outpatients. Psychopharmacology Bulletin,
    25(1), 63-67.
    Margraf, J., Ehlers, A., Roth, W. T., Clark, D. B., Sheikh, J., Agras,
    W. S., & Taylor, C. B. (1991). How "blind" are double-blind studies?
    Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 59, 184-187.
    Maynard, C. K. (1993). Comparisons of effectiveness of group
    interventions for depression in women. Archives of Psychiatric
    Nursing, 7(5), 277-283.
    Ney, P. G., Collins, C., & Spensor, C. (1986). Double blind: Double
    talk or are there ways to do better research? Medical Hypotheses, 21,
    119-126.
    Nezu, A. M. (1986). Efficacy of a social problem solving therapy
    approach for unipolar depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
    Psychology, 54(2), 196-202.
    Quality Assurance Project. (1983). A treatment outline for depressive
    disorders. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 17,
    129-146.
    Ravaris, C. L., Nies, A., Robinson, D. S., et al. (1976). A
    multiple-dose, controlled study of phenelzine in depression-anxiety
    states. Archives of General Psychiatry, 33, 347-350.
    Rehm, L. P., Kornblith, S. J., O'Hara, M. W., et al. (1981). An
    evaluation of major components in a self control therapy program for
    depression. Behavior Modification, 5(4), 459-489.
    Rickels, K., & Case, G. W. (1982). Trazodone in depressed outpatients.
    American Journal of Psychiatry, 139, 803-806.
    Rickels, K., Case, G. W., Weberlowsky, J., et al. (1981). Amoxapine
    and imipramine in the treatment of depressed outpatients: A controlled
    study. American Journal of Psychiatry, 138(1), 20-24.
    Robinson, L. A., Berman, J. S., & Neimeyer, R. A. (1990).
    Psychotherapy for the treatment of depression: A comprehensive review
    of controlled outcome research. Psychological Bulletin, 108, 30-49.
    Robinson, D. S., Nies, A., & Ravaris, C. L. (1973). The MAOI
    phenelzine in the treatment of depressive-anxiety states. Archives of
    General Psychiatry, 29, 407-413.
    Rude, S. (1986). Relative benefits of assertion or cognitive
    self-control treatment for depression as a function of proficiency in
    each domain. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 54,
    390-394.
    Schmidt, M. M., & Miller, W. R. (1983). Amount of therapist contact
    and outcome in a multidimentional depression treatment program. Acta
    Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 67, 319-332.
    Schweizer, E., Feighner, J., Mandos, L. A., & Rickels, K. (1994).
    Comparison of venlafaxine and imipramine in the acute treatment of
    major depression in outpatients. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry,
    55(3), 104-108.
    Shapiro, D. A., & Shapiro, D. (1982). Meta-analysis of comparative
    therapy outcome studies: A replication and refinement. Psychological
    Bulletin, 92, 581-604.
    Shaw, B. F. (1977). Comparison of cognitive therapy and behavior
    therapy in the treatment of depression. Journal of Consulting and
    Clinical Psychology, 45, 543-551.
    Shipley, C. R., & Fazio, A. F. (1973). Pilot study of a treatment for
    psychological depression. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 82, 372-376.
    Sloane, R. B., Staples, F. R., Cristol, A. H., Yorkston, N. J., &
    Whipple, K. (1975). Psychotherapy versus behavior therapy. Cambridge,
    MA: Harvard University Press.
    Smith, M. L., Glass, G. V., & Miller, T. I. (1980). The benefits of
    psychotherapy. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press.
    Stark, P., & Hardison, C. D. (1985). A review of multicenter
    controlled studies of fluoxetine vs. imipramine and placebo in
    outpatients with major depressive disorder. Journal of Clinical
    Psychiatry, 46, 53-58.
    Steinbrueck, S.M., Maxwell, S.E., & Howard, G.S. (1983). A
    meta-analysis of psychotherapy and drug therapy in the treatment of
    unipolar depression with adults. Journal of Consulting and Clinical
    Psychology, 51, 856-863.
    Taylor, F. G., & Marshall, W. L. (1977). Experimental analysis of a
    cognitive-behavioral therapy for depression. Cognitive Therapy and
    Research, 1(1), 59-72.
    Tyson, G. M., & Range, L. M. (1987). Gestalt dialogues as a treatment
    for depression: Time works just as well. Journal of Clinical
    Psychology, 43, 227-230.
    van der Velde, C. D. (1981). Maprotiline versus imipramine and placebo
    in neurotic depression. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 42, 138-141.
    White, K., Razani, J., Cadow, B., et al. (1984). Tranylcypromine vs.
    nortriptyline vs. placebo in depressed outpatients: a controlled
    trial. Psychopharmacology, 82, 258-262.
    Wierzbicki, M., & Bartlett, T. S. (1987). The efficacy of group and
    individual cognitive therapy for mild depression. Cognitive Therapy
    and Research, 11(3), 337-342.
    Wilson, P. H., Goldin, J. C., & Charboneau-Powis, M. (1983).
    Comparative efficacy of behavioral and cognitive treatments of
    depression. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 7(2), 111-124.
    Workman, E. A., & Short, D. D. (1993). Atypical antidepressants versus
    imipramine in the treatment of major depression: A meta-analysis.
    Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 54(1), 5-12.
    Zung, W. W. K. (1983). Review of placebo-controlled trials with
    bupropion. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 44(5), 104-114.
      _________________________________________________________________

    ^1 A reviewer suggested that because effect sizes are essentially
    z-scores in a hypothetically normal distribution, one might use
    percentile equivalents when examining the proportion of the drug
    response duplicated by the placebo response. As an example of why this
    should not be done, consider a treatment that improves intelligence by
    1.55 SDs (which is approximately at the 6^th percentile) and another
    that improves it by 1.16 SDs (which is approximately at the 12^th
    percentile). Our method indicates that the second is 75% as effective
    as the first. The reviewer's method suggests that it is only 50% as
    effective. Now let's convert this to actual IQ changes and see what
    happens. If the IQ estimates were done on conventional scales (SD =
    15), this would be equivalent to a change of 23.25 points by the first
    treatment and 17.4 points by the second. Note that the percentage
    relation is identical whether using z-scores or raw scores, because
    the z-score method simply divides both numbers by a constant.

    ^2 Instead of dividing mean differences by the pooled SDs, Joffe et
    al. (1996) used baseline SDs, when these were available, in
    calculating effect sizes. When baseline SDs were not available, which
    they reported to be the case for most of the studies they included,
    they used estimates taken from other studies. Also, they used a
    procedure derived from Hedges and Olkin (1995) to weight for
    differences in sample size, whereas we used the more straightforward
    method recommended by Hunter and Schmidt (1990).



More information about the paleopsych mailing list