<!DOCTYPE html PUBLIC "-//W3C//DTD HTML 4.01 Transitional//EN">
<html>
<head>
<meta http-equiv="Content-Type" content="text/html;charset=ISO-8859-1">
<title></title>
</head>
<body text="#000000" bgcolor="#ffffff">
Clancy's hypothesis is almost certainly the best one I have seen, and
explains the phenomena. She ignores the other side, where people report
being abducted while wide awake. A psychiatrist friend brought two
patients to my office because of my skill in hypnosis. These two
recalled being abducted while in northwest Utah, raising copper wire
from the Great Salt Lake from an old telephone line. They were unsure
about whether it was legal, so it was a bit hush-hush. My MD friend
wanted me to hypnotize them to get more info; I have never seen such
abject terror in human beings before or since. It was extremely
puzzling. I finally took a kind of agnosticism about it; I don't
believe in abductions, but I cannot explain their reactions any other
way.<br>
Lynn<br>
<br>
Premise Checker wrote:<br>
<blockquote type="cite"
cite="midPine.NEB.4.63.0508111646460.15748@panix3.panix.com">Explaining
Those Vivid Memories of Martian Kidnappers
<br>
New York Times, 5.8.9
<br>
<a class="moz-txt-link-freetext" href="http://www.nytimes.com/2005/08/09/health/09alien.html?pagewanted=print">http://www.nytimes.com/2005/08/09/health/09alien.html?pagewanted=print</a>
<br>
<br>
By [3]BENEDICT CAREY
<br>
<br>
"Abducted: How People Come to Believe They Were Kidnapped by
Aliens,"
<br>
by Susan Clancy. Harvard University Press, $22.95.
<br>
<br>
People who have memories of being abducted by aliens become hardened
<br>
skeptics, of a kind. They dismiss the procession of scientists who
<br>
explain away the memories as illusions or fantasy. They scoff at
talk
<br>
about hypnosis or the unconscious processing of Hollywood scripts.
And
<br>
they hold their ground amid snickers from a public that thinks that
<br>
they are daft or psychotic.
<br>
<br>
They are neither, it turns out, and their experiences should be
taken
<br>
as seriously as any strongly held exotic beliefs, according to Susan
<br>
Clancy, a Harvard psychologist who interviewed dozens of
<br>
self-described abductees as part of a series of memory studies over
<br>
the last several years.
<br>
<br>
In her book "Abducted," due in October, Dr. Clancy, a psychologist
at
<br>
Harvard, manages to refute and defend these believers, and along the
<br>
way provide a discussion of current research into memory, emotion
and
<br>
culture that renders abduction stories understandable, if not
<br>
believable. Although it focuses on abduction memories, the book
hints
<br>
at a larger ambition, to explain the psychology of transformative
<br>
experiences, whether supposed abductions, conversions or divine
<br>
visitations.
<br>
<br>
"Understanding why people believe weird things is important for
anyone
<br>
who wishes to know more about people - that is, humans in general,"
<br>
she writes.
<br>
<br>
Dr. Clancy's accounting for abduction memories starts with an odd
but
<br>
not uncommon experience called sleep paralysis. While in light
<br>
dream-rich REM sleep, people will in rare cases wake up for a few
<br>
moments and find themselves unable to move. Psychologists estimate
<br>
that about a fifth of people will have that experience at least
once,
<br>
during which some 5 percent will be bathed in terrifying sensations
<br>
like buzzing, full-body electrical quivers, a feeling of levitation,
<br>
at times accompanied by hallucinations of intruders.
<br>
<br>
Some of them must have an explanation as exotic as the surreal
nature
<br>
of the experience itself. Although no one has studied this group
<br>
systematically, Dr. Clancy suggests based on her interviews, that
they
<br>
tend to be people who already have some interest in the paranormal,
<br>
mystical arts and the possibility of extraterrestrial visitors.
Often
<br>
enough, their search for meaning lands them in the care of a
therapist
<br>
who uses hypnotism to elicit more details of their dreamlike
<br>
experiences.
<br>
<br>
Hypnotism is a state of deep relaxation, when people become highly
<br>
prone to suggestion, psychologists find. When encouraged under
<br>
hypnosis to imagine a vivid but entirely concocted incident - like
<br>
being awakened by loud noises - people are more likely later to
claim
<br>
the scene as a real experience, studies find.
<br>
<br>
Where, exactly, do the green figures with the wraparound eyes come
<br>
from? From the deep well of pop culture, Dr. Clancy argues, based on
a
<br>
review of the history of U.F.O. sightings, popular movies and
<br>
television programs on aliens. The first "abduction" in the United
<br>
States was dramatized in 1953, in the movie "Invaders From Mars,"
she
<br>
writes, and a rash of abduction reports followed this and other
works
<br>
on aliens, including the television series "The Outer Limits."
<br>
<br>
One such report, by a couple from New Hampshire, Betty and Barney
<br>
Hill, followed by days a particularly evocative episode of the show
in
<br>
1961. Mr. Hill's description of the aliens - with big heads and
shiny
<br>
wraparound eyes - was featured in a best-selling book about the
<br>
experience, and inspired the alien forms in Steven Spielberg's
"Close
<br>
Encounters of the Third Kind" in 1977, according to Dr. Clancy.
<br>
<br>
Thus does life imitate art, and vice versa, in a narrative hall of
<br>
mirrors in which scenes and even dialogues are recycled. Although
they
<br>
are distinct in details, abduction narratives are extremely similar
in
<br>
broad outline and often include experimentation with a sexual or
<br>
procreative subtext. "Oh! And he's opening my shirt, and - he's
going
<br>
to put that thing in my navel," says one 1970's narrative, referring
<br>
to a needle.
<br>
<br>
"I can feel them moving that thing around in my stomach, in my
body,"
<br>
the narrative, excerpted in the book, continues. The passage echoes
<br>
other abduction accounts, past and future.
<br>
<br>
In a laboratory study in 2002, Dr. Clancy and another Harvard
<br>
psychologist, Richard McNally, gave self-described abductees a
<br>
standardized word-association test intended to measure proneness to
<br>
false-memory creation. The participants studied lists of words that
<br>
were related to one another - "sugar," "candy," "sour," "bitter" -
and
<br>
to another word that was not on the list, in this case, "sweet."
<br>
<br>
When asked to recall the word lists, those with abduction memories
<br>
were more likely than a group of peers who had no such memories to
<br>
falsely recall the unlisted word. The findings suggest a
<br>
susceptibility to what are called source errors, misattributing
<br>
sources of remembered information by, say, confusing a scene from a
<br>
barely remembered movie with a dream.
<br>
<br>
In another experiment, the researchers found that recalling
abduction
<br>
memories prompted physiological changes in blood pressure and
<br>
sweat-gland activity that were higher than those seen in
<br>
post-traumatic stress syndrome. The memories produced intense
<br>
emotional trauma, and each time that occurs it deepens the certainty
<br>
that something profound really did happen.
<br>
<br>
Although no one of those elements - sleep paralysis, interest in the
<br>
paranormal, hypnotherapy, memory tricks or emotional investment - is
<br>
necessary or sufficient to create abduction memories, they tend to
<br>
cluster together in self-described abductees, Dr. Clancy finds. "In
<br>
the past, researchers have tended to concentrate on one or another"
<br>
factor, she said in an interview. "I'm saying they all play a role."
<br>
<br>
Yet abduction narratives often have another, less explicit,
dimension
<br>
that Dr. Clancy suspects may be central to their power. Consider
this
<br>
comment, from a study participant whom Dr. Clancy calls Jan, a
<br>
middle-age divorcée engaged in a quest for personal understanding:
<br>
"You know, they do walk among us on earth. They have to transform
<br>
first into a physical body, which is very painful for them. But they
<br>
do it out of love. They are here to tell us that we're all
<br>
interconnected in some way. Everything is."
<br>
<br>
At a basic level, Dr. Clancy concludes, alien abduction stories give
<br>
people meaning, a way to comprehend the many odd and dispiriting
<br>
things that buffet any life, as well as a deep sense that they are
not
<br>
alone in the universe. In this sense, abduction memories are like
<br>
transcendent religious visions, scary and yet somehow comforting
and,
<br>
at some personal psychological level, true.
<br>
<br>
Dr. Clancy said she regretted not having asked the abductees she
<br>
interviewed about religious beliefs, which were not a part of her
<br>
original research. The reader may regret that, too.
<br>
<br>
The warmth, awe and emotion of abduction stories and of those who
tell
<br>
them betray strong spiritual currents that will be familiar to
<br>
millions of people whose internal lives are animated by religious
<br>
imagery.
<br>
<br>
When it comes to sounding the depths of alien stories, a scientific
<br>
inquiry like this one may have to end with an inquiry into religio<br>
n.<br>
<pre wrap="">
<hr width="90%" size="4">
_______________________________________________
paleopsych mailing list
<a class="moz-txt-link-abbreviated" href="mailto:paleopsych@paleopsych.org">paleopsych@paleopsych.org</a>
<a class="moz-txt-link-freetext" href="http://lists.paleopsych.org/mailman/listinfo/paleopsych">http://lists.paleopsych.org/mailman/listinfo/paleopsych</a>
</pre>
</blockquote>
</body>
</html>